Cortisol administration after extinction in a fear-conditioning paradigm with traumatic film clips prevents return of fear

Alexandra H Brueckner, Johanna Lass-Hennemann, Frank H Wilhelm, Diana S Ferreira de Sá, Tanja Michael, Alexandra H Brueckner, Johanna Lass-Hennemann, Frank H Wilhelm, Diana S Ferreira de Sá, Tanja Michael

Abstract

Cortisol is a stress hormone and potent modulator of learning and memory processes. If administered after learning, cortisol can enhance memory consolidation. Yet it is unknown whether cortisol administration after fear extinction learning strengthens extinction memory. Extinction is a crucial mechanism underlying psychotherapy of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The present study examined whether extinction can be enhanced by administering cortisol after extinction training. In a registered, randomized, double-blind and placebo controlled trial, 50 healthy participants were exposed to a differential fear-conditioning paradigm with neutral faces as conditioned stimuli (CS) and traumatic film clips as unconditioned stimuli (US). They received either cortisol (n = 25) or placebo (n = 25) immediately after extinction. The cortisol group showed less fear during a return of fear manipulation (reinstatement) evidenced by attenuated fear potentiated startle responses and US-expectancy ratings than the placebo group. Results indicate that cortisol administration after fear extinction strengthens extinction memory and suggest that it might be advantageous to administer cortisol subsequent to successful exposure treatment sessions.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1. Experimental design.
Fig. 1. Experimental design.
The experiment included three appointments on three consecutive days: acquisition (day 1), extinction (day 2), and return of fear (ROF) manipulation and ROF test (day 3). Cortisol (30 mg) or placebo was administered directly after extinction training
Fig. 2. Reinforced conditioning trials.
Fig. 2. Reinforced conditioning trials.
CS duration was 8 s. Startle probe was presented 7 s after CS onset. At CS offset either a traumatic film clip (US) or a neutral film clip as control condition (CC) was presented for 16 s
Fig. 3. Behavioral outcome measures.
Fig. 3. Behavioral outcome measures.
a US-Expectancy ratings for CS+ and CS− during ROF test for cortisol and placebo group b valence ratings for CS+ and CS− during ROF test for cortisol and placebo group (means +/− standard errors)
Fig. 4. Physiological outcome measures.
Fig. 4. Physiological outcome measures.
a Startle Response to CS+ and CS− at the end of extinction and during reinstatement test for cortisol and placebo groups b skin conductance response to CS+ and CS− at the end of extinction and during reinstatement test for cortisol and placebo groups

References

    1. Cusack K, et al. Psychological treatments for adults with posttraumatic stress disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016;43:128–141. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.10.003.
    1. Schottenbauer MA, Glass CR, Arnkoff DB, Tendick V, Gray SH. Nonresponse and dropout rates in outcome studies on PTSD: review and methodological considerations. Psychiatry. 2008;71(2):134–168. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2008.71.2.134.
    1. Schnurr PP, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in women: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2007;297(8):820–830. doi: 10.1001/jama.297.8.820.
    1. Lonsdorf TB, et al. Don’t fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;77:247–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.026.
    1. Michael, T. Classical conditioning. In: Wenzel, A. E. (ed.), The SAGE Encyclopedia of Abnormal and Clinical Psychology. 660–663, SAGE Publications Inc: Thousand Oaks, California, USA (2017).
    1. Bouton ME. Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn. Mem. 2004;11(5):485–494. doi: 10.1101/lm.78804.
    1. Todd TP, Vurbic D, Bouton ME. Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of extinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2014;108:52–64. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.08.012.
    1. Vervliet B, Craske MG, Hermans D. Fear extinction and relapse: state of the art. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2013;9:215–248. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542.
    1. Bentz D, Michael T, Dominique JF, Wilhelm FH. Enhancing exposure therapy for anxiety disorders with glucocorticoids: from basic mechanisms of emotional learning to clinical applications. J. Anxiety Disord. 2010;24(2):223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.10.011.
    1. de Quervain DJ, Aerni A, Schelling G, Roozendaal B. Glucocorticoids and the regulation of memory in health and disease. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2009;30(3):358–370. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.03.002.
    1. Yang YL, Chao PK, Lu KT. Systemic and intra-amygdala administration of glucocorticoid agonist and antagonist modulate extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(5):912–924. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300899.
    1. Yang YL, Chao PK, Ro LS, Wo YYP, Lu KT. Glutamate NMDA receptors within the amygdala participate in the modulatory effect of glucocorticoids on extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(5):1042–1051. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301215.
    1. Blundell J, Blaiss CA, Lagace DC, Eisch AJ, Powell CM. Block of glucocorticoid synthesis during re-activation inhibits extinction of an established fear memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2011;95(4):453–460. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.02.006.
    1. Barrett D, Gonzalez-Lima F. Behavioral effects of metyrapone on Pavlovian extinction. Neurosci. Lett. 2004;371(2):91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.08.046.
    1. Merz CJ, Hermann A, Stark R, Wolf OT. Cortisol modifies extinction learning of recently acquired fear in men. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013;9(9):1426–1434. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst137.
    1. Bentz D, et al. Influence of stress on fear memory processes in an aversive differential conditioning paradigm in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(7):1186–1197. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.12.018.
    1. Hamacher‐Dang TC, Merz CJ, Wolf OT. Stress following extinction learning leads to a context‐dependent return of fear. Psychophysiology. 2015;52(4):489–498. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12384.
    1. de Quervain DJ, et al. Glucocorticoids enhance extinction-based psychotherapy. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108(16):6621–6625. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018214108.
    1. Lass-Hennemann J, Michael T. Endogenous cortisol levels influence exposure therapy in spider phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 2014;60:39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.06.009.
    1. Soravia L. M., Heinrichs M., Aerni A., Maroni C., Schelling G., Ehlert U., Roozendaal B., de Quervain D. J.-F. Glucocorticoids reduce phobic fear in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2006;103(14):5585–5590. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509184103.
    1. Meuret AE, et al. High cortisol awakening response and cortisol levels moderate exposure-based psychotherapy success. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;51:331–340. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.10.008.
    1. Meuret AE, et al. Timing matters: Endogenous cortisol mediates benefits from early-day psychotherapy. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;74:197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.09.008.
    1. Soravia LM, et al. Glucocorticoids enhance in vivo exposure‐based therapy of spider phobia. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31(5):429–435. doi: 10.1002/da.22219.
    1. Surís A, North C, Adinoff B, Powell CM, Greene R. Effects of exogenous glucocorticoid on combat-related PTSD symptoms. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry. 2010;22(4):274–279.
    1. Yehuda R, et al. Cortisol augmentation of a psychological treatment for warfighters with posttraumatic stress disorder: Randomized trial showing improved treatment retention and outcome. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;51:589–597. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.004.
    1. Wegerer M, Blechert J, Kerschbaum H, Wilhelm FH. Relationship between fear conditionability and aversive memories: evidence from a novel conditioned-intrusion paradigm. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(11):e79025. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079025.
    1. Streb M, Mecklinger A, Anderson MC, Lass-Hennemann J, Michael T. Memory control ability modulates intrusive memories after analogue trauma. J. Affect Disord. 2016;192:134–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.032.
    1. Graebener AH, Michael T, Holz E, Lass-Hennemann J. Repeated cortisol administration does not reduce intrusive memories–A double blind placebo controlled experimental study. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;27(11):1132–1143. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.09.001.
    1. Wegerer M, Kerschbaum H, Blechert J, Wilhelm FH. Low levels of estradiol are associated with elevated conditioned responding during fear extinction and with intrusive memories in daily life. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2014;116(Supplement C):145–154. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.10.001.
    1. Meir Drexler S, Merz CJ, Hamacher-Dang TC, Tegenthoff M, Wolf OT. Effects of Cortisol on Reconsolidation of Reactivated Fear Memories. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(13):3036–3043. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.160.
    1. Merz CJ, et al. Oral contraceptive usage alters the effects of cortisol on implicit fear learning. Horm. Behav. 2012;62(4):531–538. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.09.001.
    1. Genazzani AR, Mannella P, Simoncini T. Drospirenone and its antialdosterone properties. Climacteric. 2007;10(Suppl 1):11–18. doi: 10.1080/13697130601114891.
    1. Miedl SF, Wegerer M, Kerschbaum H, Blechert J, Wilhelm FH. Neural activity during traumatic film viewing is linked to endogenous estradiol and hormonal contraception. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018;87(Supplement C):20–26. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.10.006.
    1. Blechert J, Peyk P, Liedlgruber M, Wilhelm FH. ANSLAB: integrated multichannel peripheral biosignal processing in psychophysiological science. Behav. Res. Methods. 2016;48:1–18. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0665-1.
    1. Kunze AE, Arntz A, Kindt M. Fear conditioning with film clips: a complex associative learning paradigm. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry. 2015;47:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.11.007.
    1. Kindt M, Soeter M, Vervliet B. Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12(3):256–258. doi: 10.1038/nn.2271.
    1. Sevenster D, Beckers T, Kindt M. Retrieval per se is not sufficient to trigger reconsolidation of human fear memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2012;97(3):338–345. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.01.009.
    1. Dressendörfer R, Kirschbaum C, Rohde W, Stahl F, Strasburger C. Synthesis of a cortisol-biotin conjugate and evaluation as a tracer in an immunoassay for salivary cortisol measurement. J. Steroid Biochem. Mol. Biol. 1992;43(7):683–692. doi: 10.1016/0960-0760(92)90294-S.
    1. Soravia LM, et al. Glucocorticoids reduce phobic fear in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103(14):5585–5590. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509184103.
    1. Ludäscher Petra, Schmahl Christian, Feldmann Robert E., Kleindienst Nikolaus, Schneider Miriam, Bohus Martin. No evidence for differential dose effects of hydrocortisone on intrusive memories in female patients with complex post-traumatic stress disorder – a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2015;29(10):1077–1084. doi: 10.1177/0269881115592339.
    1. Foa EB, Kozak MJ. Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. Psychol. Bull. 1986;99(1):20. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20.
    1. Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D. & Cahill, S. P. In: Rothbaum, B. O. (ed.), Emotional Processing Theory: An Update. Pathological anxiety: Emotional Processing in Etiology and Treatment (Guilford Press: New York, 2006).
    1. Lang AJ, Craske MG. Manipulations of exposure-based therapy to reduce return of fear: a replication. Behav. Res. Ther. 2000;38(1):1–12. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00031-5.
    1. Craske MG, Treanor M, Conway CC, Zbozinek T, Vervliet B. Maximizing exposure therapy: an inhibitory learning approach. Behav. Res. Ther. 2014;58:10–23. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006.
    1. Smits JAJ, et al. d-cycloserine enhancement of exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder depends on the success of exposure sessions. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2013;47(10):1455–1461. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.06.020.
    1. Smits JAJ, et al. D-cycloserine enhancement of fear extinction is specific to successful exposure sessions: evidence from the treatment of height phobia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013;73(11):1054–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.009.
    1. Hofmann SG, d-CYCLOSERINE FOR. Treating anxiety disorders: making good exposures better and bad exposures worse. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31(3):175–177. doi: 10.1002/da.22257.
    1. LeDoux JE, Hofmann SG. The subjective experience of emotion: a fearful view. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018;19:67–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.09.011.
    1. Merz CJ, Hamacher-Dang TC, Stark R, Wolf OT, Hermann A. Neural underpinnings of cortisol effects on fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43(2):384. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.227.
    1. Blechert J, Michael T, Williams SL, Purkis HM, Wilhelm FH. When two paradigms meet: does evaluative learning extinguish in differential fear conditioning? Learn. Motiv. 2008;39(1):58–70. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2007.03.003.
    1. Vansteenwegen D, Francken G, Vervliet B, De Clercq A, Eelen P. Resistance to extinction in evaluative conditioning. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006;32(1):71.
    1. Nielsen SE, Barber SJ, Chai A, Clewett DV, Mather M. Sympathetic arousal increases a negative memory bias in young women with low sex hormone levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;62:96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.001.
    1. Rivera R, Yacobson I, Grimes D. The mechanism of action of hormonal contraceptives and intrauterine contraceptive devices. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 1999;181(5):1263–1269. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9378(99)70120-1.

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel