Dose-response effect of isometric force production on the perception of pain

Gaurav Misra, Tiffany A Paris, Derek B Archer, Stephen A Coombes, Gaurav Misra, Tiffany A Paris, Derek B Archer, Stephen A Coombes

Abstract

Isometric contractions can influence the way that we perceive pain, but conclusions on the dose-response effect of force amplitude on pain perception are limited because previous studies have not held the duration of force contractions constant while varying force amplitude. To address this issue we designed an experiment that allowed us to accurately guide the amplitude of an isometric pinch grip force contraction on a trial-by-trial basis, while a thermal pain eliciting stimulus was simultaneously delivered for the same duration to the non-contracting hand. Our results show that an increase in the amplitude of force produced by one hand corresponded with a decrease in pain perception in the opposite hand. Our observations provide novel evidence that the centralized inhibitory response that underlies analgesia is sensitive to and enhanced by stronger isometric contractions.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm.
Figure 1. Experimental paradigm.
A. The white bar represented the target force level which was set at the beginning of each trial to 5%, 25%, or 50% of each subjects’ MVC. A second red/green force bar was controlled by the subject and provided real-time visual feedback of their force production during active conditions, or moved according to a pre-recorded force trace during passive conditions. The bar was red during rest periods and green when the subject had to produce force or view the pre-recorded force trace. When the force bar turned green, the subject’s goal was to match the green bar with the white bar. Each trial was 15s long, and included 5 force pulses. Each pulse was 1.8s (bar turns green) and pulses were separated by a 1.2s rest period (bar turns red). Trial timing was the same during passive conditions but subjects were instructed to view the green bar and not produce any force. The pre-recorded force trace was collected during each participant’s practice session. Each 15s trial was followed by a 7.5s rating period. Subjects rated the level of stimulation they experienced during the previous 15s trial. Ratings were made using a button box to control a cursor on a visual analog scale (VAS) presented on the screen. B. Example force traces (black lines – left y-axis) and temperature traces (red lines – right y-axis) from active trials from one subject for the hot condition at 5%, 25%, and 50% of MVC.
Figure 2. Analgesic score (passive condition –…
Figure 2. Analgesic score (passive condition – active condition) are shown for each target force level for the hot condition.
Analgesic scores at 50% were greater than at 5% of MVC. Error bars represent SE.
Figure 3. Mean amplitude and variability of…
Figure 3. Mean amplitude and variability of force production during active trials.
Figures A-C show mean force amplitude, mean standard deviation, and mean coefficient of variation of force production during active trials for each temperature and each target MVC level. Error bars represent SE.

References

    1. Naugle KM, Fillingim RB, Riley JL 3rd (2012) A meta-analytic review of the hypoalgesic effects of exercise. The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society 13: 1139–1150.
    1. Koltyn KF (2000) Analgesia following exercise: a review. Sports medicine 29: 85–98.
    1. Koltyn KF, Umeda M (2006) Exercise, hypoalgesia and blood pressure. Sports medicine 36: 207–214.
    1. Koltyn KF, Knauf MT, Brellenthin AG (2013) Temporal summation of heat pain modulated by isometric exercise. Eur J Pain 17: 1005–1011.
    1. Koltyn KF, Umeda M (2007) Contralateral attenuation of pain after short-duration submaximal isometric exercise. The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society 8: 887–892.
    1. Staud R, Robinson ME, Price DD (2005) Isometric exercise has opposite effects on central pain mechanisms in fibromyalgia patients compared to normal controls. Pain 118: 176–184.
    1. Hoeger Bement MK, Dicapo J, Rasiarmos R, Hunter SK (2008) Dose response of isometric contractions on pain perception in healthy adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc 40: 1880–1889.
    1. Ring C, Edwards L, Kavussanu M (2008) Effects of isometric exercise on pain are mediated by blood pressure. Biological psychology 78: 123–128.
    1. Kosek E, Lundberg L (2003) Segmental and plurisegmental modulation of pressure pain thresholds during static muscle contractions in healthy individuals. European journal of pain 7: 251–258.
    1. Paris TA, Misra G, Archer DB, Coombes SA (2013) Effects of a Force Production Task and a Working Memory Task on Pain Perception. J Pain 14: 1492–1501.
    1. Spielberger CD (1983) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). PaloAlto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
    1. Beck AT, Steer RA (1987) Beck Depression Inventory manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corp.
    1. McCracken LM, Zayfert C, Gross RT (1992) The Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale: development and validation of a scale to measure fear of pain. Pain 50: 67–73.
    1. Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J (1995) The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and Validation. Psychological Assessment 7: 524–532.
    1. Kori SH, Miller RP, Todd DD (1990) Kinisophobia: A new view of chronic pain behavior. Pain Management 3: 35–43.
    1. Buhle JT, Stevens BL, Friedman JJ, Wager TD (2012) Distraction and placebo: two separate routes to pain control. Psychological science 23: 246–253.
    1. Buhle J, Wager TD (2010) Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task. Pain 149: 19–26.
    1. Neely KA, Coombes SA, Planetta PJ, Vaillancourt DE (2013) Segregated and overlapping neural circuits exist for the production of static and dynamic precision grip force. Human brain mapping 34: 698–712.
    1. Lodha N, Coombes SA, Cauraugh JH (2012) Bimanual isometric force control: asymmetry and coordination evidence post stroke. Clinical neurophysiology : official journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology 123: 787–795.
    1. Coombes SA, Corcos DM, Pavuluri MN, Vaillancourt DE (2012) Maintaining force control despite changes in emotional context engages dorsomedial prefrontal and premotor cortex. Cerebral cortex 22: 616–627.
    1. Coombes SA, Corcos DM, Vaillancourt DE (2011) Spatiotemporal tuning of brain activity and force performance. Neuroimage 54: 2226–2236.
    1. Coombes SA, Corcos DM, Sprute L, Vaillancourt DE (2010) Selective regions of the visuomotor system are related to gain-induced changes in force error. Journal of neurophysiology 103: 2114–2123.
    1. Umeda M, Newcomb LW, Ellingson LD, Koltyn KF (2010) Examination of the dose-response relationship between pain perception and blood pressure elevations induced by isometric exercise in men and women. Biological psychology 85: 90–96.
    1. Henneman E (1957) Relation between size of neurons and their susceptibility to discharge. Science 126: 1345–1347.
    1. Adrian ED, Bronk DW (1929) The discharge of impulses in motor nerve fibres: Part II. The frequency of discharge in reflex and voluntary contractions. The Journal of physiology 67: i3–151.
    1. Spraker MB, Corcos DM, Kurani AS, Prodoehl J, Swinnen SP, et al. (2011) Specific cerebellar regions are related to force amplitude and rate of force development. Neuroimage.
    1. Spraker MB, Yu H, Corcos DM, Vaillancourt DE (2007) Role of individual basal ganglia nuclei in force amplitude generation. J Neurophysiol 98: 821–834.
    1. Lodha N, Naik SK, Coombes SA, Cauraugh JH (2010) Force control and degree of motor impairments in chronic stroke. Clin Neurophysiol 121: 1952–1961.
    1. Vaillancourt DE, Russell DM (2002) Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in continuous force production. Exp Brain Res 145: 275–285.
    1. Taylor AM, Christou EA, Enoka RM (2003) Multiple features of motor-unit activity influence force fluctuations during isometric contractions. Journal of neurophysiology 90: 1350–1361.
    1. Hirayama A, Saitoh Y, Kishima H, Shimokawa T, Oshino S, et al. (2006) Reduction of intractable deafferentation pain by navigation-guided repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the primary motor cortex. Pain 122: 22–27.
    1. Yezierski RP, Gerhart KD, Schrock BJ, Willis WD (1983) A further examination of effects of cortical stimulation on primate spinothalamic tract cells. Journal of neurophysiology 49: 424–441.
    1. Senapati AK, Huntington PJ, Peng YB (2005) Spinal dorsal horn neuron response to mechanical stimuli is decreased by electrical stimulation of the primary motor cortex. Brain research 1036: 173–179.
    1. Sprenger C, Eippert F, Finsterbusch J, Bingel U, Rose M, et al. (2012) Attention modulates spinal cord responses to pain. Current biology : CB 22: 1019–1022.

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel