Making Inferences: Comprehension of Physical Causality, Intentionality, and Emotions in Discourse by High-Functioning Older Children, Adolescents, and Adults with Autism

Kimberly E Bodner, Christopher R Engelhardt, Nancy J Minshew, Diane L Williams, Kimberly E Bodner, Christopher R Engelhardt, Nancy J Minshew, Diane L Williams

Abstract

Studies investigating inferential reasoning in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have focused on the ability to make socially-related inferences or inferences more generally. Important variables for intervention planning such as whether inferences depend on physical experiences or the nature of social information have received less consideration. A measure of bridging inferences of physical causation, mental states, and emotional states was administered to older children, adolescents, and adults with and without ASD. The ASD group had more difficulty making inferences, particularly related to emotional understanding. Results suggest that individuals with ASD may not have the stored experiential knowledge that specific inferences depend upon or have difficulties accessing relevant experiences due to linguistic limitations. Further research is needed to tease these elements apart.

Conflict of interest statement

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Association between age and PIT weighted total scores by group diagnosis
Figure 2
Figure 2
Association between age and PIT weighted total scores by low and high VIQ scores
Figure 3
Figure 3
Association between age and number of correct other-ToM responses by low and high VIQ scores

References

    1. Algina J, Keselman HJ. Approximate confidence intervals for effect sizes. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 2003;63:537–553.
    1. American Psychological Association. Publication manual of the American Psychological Association. 6. Washington, DC: Author; 2010.
    1. Anderson D. A few quotes regarding hypothesis testing. 1997 Retrieved from .
    1. Arciuli J, Stevens K, Trembath D, Simpson IC. The relationship between parent report of adaptive behavior and direct assessment of reading ability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2013;56(6):1837–1844.
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition. 1985;21:37–46.
    1. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test Revised Version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger Syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiat. 2001;42:241–251.
    1. Berger JO, Berry DA. Statistical analysis and the illusion of objectivity. American Scientist. 1988;76:159–165.
    1. Bowler D. “Theory of Mind” in Asperger’s Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1992;33:877–893.
    1. Bowler D. Reaction times to mental state and non-mental state questions in false belief tasks by high-functioning individuals with autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;6:160–165.
    1. Brent E, Rios P, Happé F, Charman T. Performance of children with autism spectrum disorder on advanced theory of mind tasks. Autism. 2004;8:283–299.
    1. Brown HM, Oram-Cardy J, Johnson A. A meta-analysis of the reading comprehension skills of individuals on the autism spectrum. Journal of autism and developmental disorders. 2013;43(4):932–955.
    1. Cumming G. Understanding the new statistics: Effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis. New York, NY: Routledge; 2012.
    1. Cumming G. The new statistics: Why and how. Psychological Science. 2014;25:7–29.
    1. Cumming G, Fidler F. Confidence Intervals: Better answers to better questions. Zeitschrift fuer Psychologie/Journal of Psychology. 2009;217:15–26.
    1. Dennis M, Lazenby AL, Lockyer L. Inferential language in high-function children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2001;31:47–54.
    1. Edwards W, Lindman H, Savage LJ. Bayesian statistical inference for psychological research. Psychological Review. 1963;70(3):193–242.
    1. Gallistel CR. The importance of proving the null. Psychological Review. 2009;116(2):439–453.
    1. Graesser AC, Singer M, Trabasso T. Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological review. 1994;101(3):371.
    1. Groen WB, Zwiers MP, van der Gaag RJ, Buitelaar JK. The phenotype and neural correlates of language in autism: An integrative review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32:1416–1425.
    1. Happé FGE. An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24:129–154.
    1. Hoijtink H, Klugkist I, Boelen P. Bayesian evaluation of informative hypotheses that are of practical value for social scientists. New York: Springer; 2008.
    1. Hollingshead AB. Four factor index of social status. Yale University, Department of Sociology; New Haven, CT: 1975.
    1. Huemer SV, Mann V. A comprehensive profile of decoding and comprehension in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40(4):485–493.
    1. Jeffreys H. Theory of probability. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1961.
    1. Jolliffe T, Baron-Cohen S. The strange stories test: a replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1999;29:395–404.
    1. Kaland N, Møller-Nielsen A, Smith L, Mortensen EL, Callesen K, Gottlieb D. The strange stories test: A replication study of children and adolescents with Asperger syndrome. European child & adolescent psychiatry. 2005;14(2):73–82.
    1. Kass RE. Bayes factors in practice. The Statistician. 1993;42:551–560.
    1. Kass RE, Raftery AE. Bayes factors. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1995;90:773–795.
    1. Kirk RE. The importance of effect magnitude. In: Davis SF, editor. Handbook of research methods in experimental psychology. Malden, MA: Blackwell; 2003. pp. 83–105.
    1. Leinonen E, Kerbel D. Relevance theory and pragmatic impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 1999;34(4):367–390.
    1. Le Sourn-Bissaoui S, Caillies S, Gierski F, Motte J. Inference processing in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: Relationship with theory of mind abilities. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2009;3(3):797–808.
    1. Lewis FM, Murdoch BE, Woodyatt GC. Communicative competence and metalinguistic ability: Performance by children and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37:1525–1538.
    1. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavone PC, et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30:205–223.
    1. Lord C, Rutter M, LeCouteur AL. Autism diagnostic interview revised. A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24:659–685.
    1. Loukusa S, Leinonen E, Kuusikko S, Jussila K, Mattila ML, Ryder N, Ebeling H, Moilanen I. Use of context in pragmatic language comprehension by children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37:1049–1059.
    1. Mason RA, Williams DL, Kana RK, Minshew NJ, Just MA. Theory of mind disruption and recruitment of the right hemisphere during narrative comprehension in autism. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:269–280.
    1. Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Siegel DJ. Speech and language in high-functioning autistic individuals. Neuropsychology. 1995;9:255–261.
    1. Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Siegel DJ. Neuropsychological functioning in ASD: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1997;3:303–316.
    1. Morey RD, Rouder JN. BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.7. 2014 .
    1. Morey RD, Rouder JN, Verhagen J, Wagenmakers E-J. Why hypothesis tests are essential for psychological science: A comment on Cumming 2014. Psychological Science. n.d;25:1289–1290.
    1. Myung IJ, Pitt MA. Applying Occam’s razor in modeling cognition: A Bayesian approach. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 1997;4(1):79–95.
    1. Norbury CF, Bishop DV. Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: a comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2002;37:227–251.
    1. Perner J, Wimmer H. “John thinks that Mary thinks that…” Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5–10 year old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1985;39:437–471.
    1. Raftery AE. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology. 1995;25:111–164.
    1. Ricketts J, Jones CRG, Happé F, Charman T. Reading comprehension in autism spectrum disorders: The role of oral language and social functioning. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43:807–816.
    1. Rosnow RL, Rosenthal R. Effect sizes: Why, when, and how to use them. Zeitschrift fuer Psychologie/Journal of Psychology. 2009;217:6–14.
    1. Rouder JN, Speckman PL, Sun D, Morey RD, Iverson G. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2009;16(2):225–237.
    1. Saldaña D, Frith U. Do readers with autism make bridging inferences from world knowledge? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2007;96:310–319.
    1. Sebastian CL, Fontaine NM, Bird G, Blakemore SJ, De Brito SA, McCrory EJ, Viding E. Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7:53–63.
    1. Shamay-Tsoory SG. The neural bases for empathy. The Neuroscientist. 2011;17:18–24.
    1. Singer M. Validation in reading comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2013;22(5):361–366.
    1. Social Security Online. Popular names by birth year: popularity in 2005. 2008 May; Retrieved from Social Security Administration website:
    1. Snyder L, Caccamise D. Comprehension processes for expository text: Building meaning and making sense. In: Nippold M, Scott CM, editors. Expository discourse in children, adolescents, and adults. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2010. pp. 13–39.
    1. Sperber D, Wilson D. Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind & Language. 2002;17:3–23.
    1. Tager-Flusberg H, Joseph RM. How language facilitates the acquisition of false-belief understanding in children with autism. In: Astington JW, Baird JA, editors. Why language matters for theory of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 298–318.
    1. Wagenmakers EJ. A practical solution n to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2007;14(5):779–804.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1999.
    1. Wiig EH, Secord W. Test of Language Competence-Expanded Edition (TLC-E) San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1989.
    1. Williams DL, Cherkassky VL, Mason RA, Keller TA, Minshew NJ, Just MA. Brain function differences in language processing in children and adults with autism. Autism Research. 2013;6:288–302.
    1. Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ. Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: Further evidence for disordered complex information processing. Child Neuropsychology. 2006;12:279–298.

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel