Promoting prosocial behavior and self-regulatory skills in preschool children through a mindfulness-based Kindness Curriculum

Lisa Flook, Simon B Goldberg, Laura Pinger, Richard J Davidson, Lisa Flook, Simon B Goldberg, Laura Pinger, Richard J Davidson

Abstract

Self-regulatory abilities are robust predictors of important outcomes across the life span, yet they are rarely taught explicitly in school. Using a randomized controlled design, the present study investigated the effects of a 12-week mindfulness-based Kindness Curriculum (KC) delivered in a public school setting on executive function, self-regulation, and prosocial behavior in a sample of 68 preschool children. The KC intervention group showed greater improvements in social competence and earned higher report card grades in domains of learning, health, and social-emotional development, whereas the control group exhibited more selfish behavior over time. Interpretation of effect sizes overall indicate small to medium effects favoring the KC group on measures of cognitive flexibility and delay of gratification. Baseline functioning was found to moderate treatment effects with KC children initially lower in social competence and executive functioning demonstrating larger gains in social competence relative to the control group. These findings, observed over a relatively short intervention period, support the promise of this program for promoting self-regulation and prosocial behavior in young children. They also support the need for future investigation of program implementation across diverse settings.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Treatment effects on sharing behavior. Over time the control group kept more stickers for themselves (shared less with others) on average across all trials relative to KC group. Standard error bars displayed. Plot displays results of RMANOVA analysis controlling for sharing at baseline.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Group differences in end of year school grades. Kindness Curriculum group earned higher marks than control group across three subject areas.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Individual differences in baseline inhibitory control predict response to intervention. Baseline inhibitory control (Flanker) predicts change (post - pre) in social competence. Mean change in social competence is displayed separately for KC and control group for bottom quartile, middle half, and top quartile performances on Flanker at baseline (quartiles computed for display purposes only). Children with lower inhibitory control in the KC group increased more in social competence compared to the control group. Interaction significant at p < .001

References

    1. Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology. 2002;8(2):71–82. .
    1. Barkley RA. The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review. 2001;11:1–29. .
    1. Becker B. Synthesizing standardized mean-change measures. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 1988;41:257–278. .
    1. Bierman KL, Nix RL, Greenberg MT, Blair C, Domitrovich CE. Executive functions and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and meditation in the Head Start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:821–843. .
    1. Black DS, Milam J, Sussman S. Sitting-meditation interventions among youth: A review of treatment efficacy. Pediatrics. 2009;124:e532–e541. .
    1. Blair C. School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist. 2002;57(2):111–127. .
    1. Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development. 2007;78(2):647–663. .
    1. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ; 1988. .
    1. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Teacher - Social Competence Scale. 1995 Retrieved from the Fast Track Project Web site, .
    1. Davidson RJ, Dunne J, Eccles JS, Engle A, Greenberg M, Jennings P, Vago D. Contemplative practices and mental training: Prospects for American education. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6(2):146–153. .
    1. Davidson RJ, McEwen BS. Social influences on neuroplasticity: Stress and interventions to promote well-being. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15(5):689–695. .
    1. Diamond A. Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In: Stuss DT, Knight RT, editors. Principles of frontal lobe function. London, UK: Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 466–503. .
    1. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333(6045):959–964. .
    1. Eigsti I, Zayas V, Mischel W, Shoda Y, Ayduk O, Dadlani MB, Casey BJ. Predicting cognitive control from preschool to late adolescence and young adulthood. Psychological Science. 2006;17(6):478–484. .
    1. Eisenberg N, Spinrad T, Smith C. The regulation of emotion. Mahwah, NJ US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2004. Emotion-related regulation: Its conceptualization, relations to social functioning, and socialization; pp. 277–306.
    1. Flook L, Smalley SL, Kitil MJ, Galla BM, Kaiser-Greenland S, Locke J, Kasari C. Effects of mindful awareness practices on executive functions in elementary school children. Journal of Applied School Psychology. 2010;26(1):70–95. .
    1. Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134(1):31–60. .
    1. Greenberg MT, Weissberg RP, O’Brien MU, Zins JE, Fredericks L, Resnik H, Elias MJ. Enhancing school-based prevention and youth development through coordinated social, emotional, and academic learning. American Psychologist. 2003;58(6–7):466–474. .
    1. Heckman JJ. The economics of inequality: The value of early childhood education. American Educator. 2011;35(1):31–35. 47. Retrieved from ERIC database.
    1. Lutz A, Brefczynski-Lewis JA, Johnston T, Davidson RJ. Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: Effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE. 2008;3(3):e1897. 26 .
    1. Lutz A, Slagter HA, Dunne JD, Davidson RJ. Meditation and the regulation of attention and emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2008;12(4):163–169. .
    1. MacLean KA, Ferrer E, Aichele SR, Bridwell DA, Zanesco AP, Jacobs TL, Saron CD. Intensive meditation training improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention. Psychological Science. 2010;21:829–839. .
    1. McCloskey G, Perkins L, Van Divner B. Assessment and intervention for executive function difficulties. New York, NY US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2009.
    1. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Peake PK. The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54(4):687–696. .
    1. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML. Delay of gratification in children. Science. 1989;244(4907):933–938. .
    1. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41:49–100. .
    1. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, Caspi A. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108(7):2693–2698. .
    1. Napoli M, Krech PR, Holley LC. Mindfulness Training for Elementary School Students: The Attention Academy. Journal of Applied School Psychology. 2005;21(1):99–125. .
    1. Neville HJ, Stevens C, Pakulak E, Bell TA, Fanning J, Klein S, Isbell E. Family-based training program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status preschoolers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(29):12138–12143. .
    1. Prencipe A, Zelazo P. Development of affective decision making for self and other. Psychological Science. 2005;16(7):501–505. .
    1. Raver CC. Low-income children’s self-regulation in the classroom: Scientific inquiry for social change. American Psychologist. 2012;67(8):681–689. .
    1. Snijders TA, Bosker RJ. Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advance multilevel modeling. 2nd ed. London: Sage; 2012.
    1. Shoda Y, Mischel W, Peake PK. Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology. 1990;26(6):978–986. .
    1. Spinrad T, Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Fabes R, Valiente C, Shepard S, Guthrie IK. Relation of emotion-related regulation to children’s social competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion. 2006;6(3):498–510. .
    1. Weng HY, Fox AS, Shackman AJ, Stodola DE, Caldwell JZK, Olson MC, Rogers GM, Davidson RJ. Compassion training alters altruism and neural responses to suffering. Psychological Science. 2013;24(7) .
    1. Zelazo PD, Anderson JE, Richler J, Wallner-Allen K, Beaumont JL, Weintraub S. II. NIH TOOLBOX COGNITION BATTERY (CB): MEASURING EXECUTIVE FUNCTION AND ATTENTION. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2013;78:16–33.
    1. Zelazo P, Carlson S, Kesek A. Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. 2nd ed. Cambridge, MA US: MIT Press; 2008. The development of executive function in childhood; pp. 553–574.

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel