Cognitive Restructuring Before Versus After Exposure: Effect on Expectancy and Outcome in Individuals With Claustrophobia

Kirstyn L Krause, Naomi Koerner, Martin M Antony, Kirstyn L Krause, Naomi Koerner, Martin M Antony

Abstract

Maximizing the discrepancy between expected and actual outcomes during exposure (i.e., expectancy violation) is thought to optimize inhibitory learning. The current study examined Craske et al.'s suggestion that engaging in cognitive restructuring (CR) before exposure prematurely reduces expectancy and mitigates outcomes. Participants (N = 93) with claustrophobia were randomly assigned to either 15 minutes of CR before exposure (CR Before) or 15 minutes of CR after exposure (CR After). Although the CR Before condition experienced greater expectancy reduction before exposure than the CR After condition, both groups experienced similar overall expectancy reduction by the end of the intervention. Groups experienced similar gains, with large significant improvement at posttreatment and follow-up. Results suggest that both cognitive therapy and exposure therapy lead to expectancy reduction, but that the order of these interventions does not impact outcome. Clinicaltrials.org registration #NCT03628105.

Keywords: cognitive behavior therapy; cognitive restructuring; expectancy; expectancy violation; exposure therapy; inhibitory learning model.

Conflict of interest statement

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Martin Antony has published a number of books on cognitive and behavioral treatments for specific phobias and other anxiety related disorders, and regularly presents workshops on related topics. Kirstyn Krause and Naomi Koerner have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Participant flow. Note. CR = cognitive restructuring.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Visual representation of procedure. Note. DASS-21 = Depression Anxiety Stress Scales; CLQ = Claustrophobia Questionnaire; CGCQ = Claustrophobia General Cognition Questionnaire; BAT = Behavioral Approach Test; CR = cognitive restructuring.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Estimated marginal means for expected feared outcomes. Note. There were no significant differences between groups at Time 1 or Time 2. Asterisk indicates a significant difference between groups at Time 3. Error bars represent 95% Confidence Interval. Time represents expected feared outcome ratings: 1 = before psychoeducation, 2 = after psychoeducation/before the preexposure thought record versus filler task manipulation, 3 = after the preexposure thought record versus filler task manipulation. ANOVA = analysis of variance; CR = cognitive restructuring.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Estimated marginal means for treatment gains. Note. There were no significant differences between groups at pretreatment, posttreatment, or follow-up. Error bars represent 95% Confidence Interval. BAT = Behavioral Approach Test; CR = cognitive restructuring; ANOVA = analysis of variance.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Change in expected feared outcomes from Time 3 to Time 9. Note. Error bars represent 95% Confidence Interval. Individual data points are offset from each time to reduce overplotting. Time represents expected feared outcome ratings: 3 = after the preexposure thought record versus filler task manipulation/before the first exposure trial, 4–8 = before the remaining five exposure trials during the intervention appointment; 9 = before the exposure trial during the follow-up appointment. CR = cognitive restructuring.

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Author.
    1. Antony M. M., Bieling P. J., Cox B. J., Enns M. W., Swinson R. P. (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176–181. 10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176
    1. Antony M. M., Craske M. G., Barlow D. H. (2006). Mastering your fears and phobias workbook (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.
    1. Barlow D. H., Craske M. G. (2007). Mastery of your anxiety and panic: Workbook (4th ed.). Oxford University Press.
    1. Benito K. G., Machan J., Freeman J. B., Garcia A. M., Walther M., Frank H., Wellen B., Stewart E., Edmunds J., Kemp J., Sapyta J., Franklin M. (2018). Measuring fear change within exposures: Functionally-defined habituation predicts outcome in three randomized controlled trials for pediatric OCD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86(7), 615–630. 10.1037/ccp0000315
    1. Bjork R. A., Bjork E. L. (2006). Optimizing treatment and instruction: Implications of a new theory of disuse. In Nilsson L.-G., Ohta N. (Eds.), Memory and society: Psychological perspectives (pp. 116–140). Psychology Press.
    1. Blanca M. J., Alarcón R., Arnau J., Bono R., Bendayan R. (2018). Effect of variance ratio on ANOVA robustness: Might 1.5 be the limit? Behavior Research Methods, 50, 937–962. 10.3758/s13428-017-0918-2
    1. Boddez Y., Baeyens F., Luyten L., Vansteenwegen D., Hermans D., Beckers T. (2013). Rating data are underrated: Validity of US expectancy in human fear conditioning. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44(2), 201–206. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.08.003
    1. Bouton M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 80–99. 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80
    1. Butcher J. N., Graham J. R., Tellegen A., Kaemmer B. (1989). Manual for the restandardized Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory: MMPI-2. An administrative and interpretive guide. University of Minnesota Press.
    1. Carpenter J. K., Pinaire M., Hofmann S. G. (2019). From extinction learning to anxiety treatment: Mind the gap. Brain Sciences, 9(7), 164. 10.3390/brainsci9070164
    1. Clark D. A. (1995). Perceived limitations of standard cognitive therapy: A consideration of efforts to revise Beck’s theory and therapy. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 9(3), 153–172. 10.1891/0889-8391.9.3.153
    1. Craske M. G., Kircanski K., Zelikowsky M., Mystkowski J., Chowdhury N., Baker A. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(1), 5–27. 10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003
    1. Craske M. G., Treanor M., Conway C. C., Zbozinek T., Vervliet B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 58, 10–23. 10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006
    1. Davis T. E., III, Ollendick T. H., Öst L.-G. (Eds.). (2012). Intensive one-session treatment of specific phobias. Springer.
    1. Deacon B., Kemp J. J., Dixon L. J., Sy J. T., Farrell N. R., Zhang A. R. (2013). Maximizing the efficacy of interoceptive exposure by optimizing inhibitory learning: A randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(9), 588–596. 10.1016/j.brat.2013.06.006
    1. Deacon B. J., Sy J. T., Lickel J. J., Nelson E. A. (2010). Does the judicious use of safety behaviors improve the efficacy and acceptability of exposure therapy for claustrophobic fear? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(1), 71–80. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.10.004
    1. Devilly G. J., Borkovec T. D. (2000). Psychometric properties of the credibility/expectancy questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 31(2), 73–86. 10.1016/s0005-7916(00)00012-4
    1. Field A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (2nd ed.). SAGE Publications.
    1. Foa E. B., Huppert J. D., Cahill S. P. (2006). Emotional processing theory: An update. In Rothbaum B. O. (Ed.), Pathological anxiety: Emotional processing in etiology and treatment (pp. 3–24). Guilford Press.
    1. Greenberger D., Padesky C. A. (2016). Mind over mood: Change how you feel by changing the way you think (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.
    1. Jakobsen J. C., Gluud C., Wetterslev J., Winkel P. (2017). When and how should multiple imputation be used for handling missing data in randomised clinical trials – A practical guide with flowcharts. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 17(1), 162. 10.1186/s12874-017-0442-1
    1. Landis J. R., Koch G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159–174. 10.2307/2529310
    1. Lovibond P. F., Lovibond S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U
    1. McCabe R. E., Milosevic I., Rowa K., Shnaider P., Pawluk E. J., Antony M. M.; The DART Working Group. (2017). Diagnostic Assessment Research Tool (DART). Joseph’s Healthcare Hamilton/McMaster University.
    1. Öst L.-G., Alm T., Brandberg M., Breitholtz E. (2001). One vs five sessions of exposure and five sessions of cognitive therapy in the treatment of claustrophobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39(2), 167–183. 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00176-x
    1. Podina I. R., Vîslă A., Fodor L. A., Flückiger C. (2019). Is there a sleeper effect of exposure-based vs. cognitive-only intervention for anxiety disorders? A longitudinal multilevel meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 73, 101774. 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101774
    1. Quené H., van Den Bergh H. (2004). On multi-level modeling of data from repeated measures designs: A tutorial. Speech Communication, 43(1–2), 103–121. 10.1016/j.specom.2004.02.004
    1. Rachman S. (1974). The meanings of fear. Penguin Education.
    1. Rachman S. (1994). The overprediction of fear: A review. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(7), 683–690. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90025-6
    1. Radomsky A. S., Ouimet A. J., Ashbaugh A. R., Paradis M. R., Lavoie S. L., O’Connor K. P. (2006). Psychometric properties of the French and English versions of the Claustrophobia Questionnaire (CLQ). Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(6), 818–828. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.01.002
    1. Radomsky A. S., Rachman S., Thordarson D. S., McIsaac H. K., Teachman B. A. (2001). The Claustrophobia questionnaire. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 15(4), 287–297. 10.1016/s0887-6185(01)00064-0
    1. Rescorla R. A., Wagner A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black A. H., Prokasy W. F. (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). Appleton-Century-Crofts.
    1. Rupp C., Doebler P., Ehring T., Vossbeck-Elsebusch A. N. (2017). Emotional processing theory put to test: A meta-analysis on the association between process and outcome measures in exposure therapy. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 24(3), 697–711. 10.1002/cpp.2039
    1. Schneider L. H., Pawluk E. J., Milosevic I., Shnaider P., Rowa K., Antony M. M., Musielak N., McCabe R. E. (2021). The diagnostic assessment research tool in action: A preliminary evaluation of a semistructured diagnostic interview for DSM-5 disorders. Psychological Assessment. Advance online publication. 10.1037/pas0001059

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel