Effects of changes in water intake on mood of high and low drinkers

Nathalie Pross, Agnès Demazières, Nicolas Girard, Romain Barnouin, Déborah Metzger, Alexis Klein, Erica Perrier, Isabelle Guelinckx, Nathalie Pross, Agnès Demazières, Nicolas Girard, Romain Barnouin, Déborah Metzger, Alexis Klein, Erica Perrier, Isabelle Guelinckx

Abstract

Objective: To evaluate the effects of a change in water intake on mood and sensation in 22 habitual high-volume (HIGH; 2-4 L/d) and 30 low-volume (LOW; <1.2 L/d) drinkers who were asked to respectively decrease and increase their daily water intake.

Method: During baseline HIGH consumed 2.5 L and LOW 1 L of water/day. During 3 controlled intervention days HIGH's water intake was restricted to 1 L/day whereas LOW's was increased to 2.5 L water/day. Several mood scales (Bond & Lader Visual Analog Scale (VAS), Profile of Mood States, Karolinska Sleepiness Scale, Thirst & Emotional VAS) were administered at different time points during the study. ANOVA including intervention, time point and intervention by time point as fixed effects on mean values (i.e.; baseline data vs. mean of 3 intervention days) for each mood scale was performed.

Results: At baseline HIGH and LOW were comparable in mood state, except for thirst scores (estimate = 17.16, p<0.001) and POMS depression-dejection scores (estimate = 0.55, p<0.05) which were both higher in the HIGH vs. LOW. In HIGH the restricted water intake resulted in a significant increase in thirst (p<0.001) and a decrease in contentedness (p<0.05), calmness (p<0.01), positive emotions (p<0.05) and vigor/activity (p<0.001). In LOW, increased water consumption resulted in a significant decrease in fatigue/inertia (p<0.001), confusion/bewilderment (p = 0.05) and thirst (p<0.001) and a trend to lower sleepiness (p = 0.07) compared to baseline.

Conclusion: Increasing water intake has beneficial effects in LOW, especially sleep/wake feelings, whereas decreasing water intake has detrimental effects on HIGH's mood. These deleterious effects in HIGH were observed in some sleep/wake moods as well as calmness, satisfaction and positive emotions.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have the following interests: This study was funded by Danone Research and conducted by Forenap. AD, NG, RB, DM, and NP were employees of Forenap, where the trial was conducted. AK, EP, and IG are employees of Danone Research. NP is an occasional consultant for Danone Research. There are no patents, products in development or marketed products to declare. This does not alter the authors′ adherence to all the PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.

Figures

Figure 1. Investigational schedule.
Figure 1. Investigational schedule.
Note: X corresponds to the mood and sensations assessments (i.e.; Thirst VAS, KSS, eVAS, POMS, Bond and Lader VAS).
Figure 2. Plots of thirst scores (mean…
Figure 2. Plots of thirst scores (mean ± SEM) over time according to groups and days.

References

    1. Benton D (2011) Dehydration influences mood and cognition: a plausible hypothesis? Nutrients 3: 555–573.
    1. Adam GE, Carter R III, Cheuvront SN, Merullo DJ, Castellani JW, et al. (2008) Hydration effects on cognitive performance during military tasks in temperate and cold environments. Physiol Behav 93: 748–756.
    1. Lieberman HR, Bathalon GP, Falco CM, Kramer FM, Morgan CA III, et al. (2005) Severe decrements in cognition function and mood induced by sleep loss, heat, dehydration, and undernutrition during simulated combat. Biol Psychiatry 57: 422–429.
    1. Patel AV, Mihalik JP, Notebaert AJ, Guskiewicz KM, Prentice WE (2007) Neuropsychological performance, postural stability, and symptoms after dehydration. J Athl Train 42: 66–75.
    1. Bandelow S, Maughan R, Shirreffs S, Ozgunen K, Kurdak S, et al. (2010) The effects of exercise, heat, cooling and rehydration strategies on cognitive function in football players. Scand J Med Sci Sports 20 Suppl 3148–160.
    1. Szinnai G, Schachinger H, Arnaud MJ, Linder L, Keller U (2005) Effect of water deprivation on cognitive-motor performance in healthy men and women. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 289: R275–R280.
    1. Petri NM, Dropulic N, Kardum G (2006) Effects of voluntary fluid intake deprivation on mental and psychomotor performance. Croat Med J 47: 855–861.
    1. Shirreffs SM, Merson SJ, Fraser SM, Archer DT (2004) The effects of fluid restriction on hydration status and subjective feelings in man. Br J Nutr 91: 951–958.
    1. Ganio MS, Armstrong LE, Casa DJ, McDermott BP, Lee EC, et al. (2011) Mild dehydration impairs cognitive performance and mood of men. Br J Nutr 106: 1535–1543.
    1. Armstrong LE, Ganio MS, Casa DJ, Lee EC, McDermott BP, et al. (2012) Mild dehydration affects mood in healthy young women. J Nutr 142: 382–388.
    1. Pross N, Demazieres A, Girard N, Barnouin R, Santoro F, et al. (2013) Influence of progressive fluid restriction on mood and physiological markers of dehydration in women. Br J Nutr 109: 313–321.
    1. Rogers PJ, Kainth A, Smit HJ (2001) A drink of water can improve or impair mental performance depending on small differences in thirst. Appetite 36: 57–58.
    1. Neave N, Scholey AB, Emmett JR, Moss M, Kennedy DO, et al. (2001) Water ingestion improves subjective alertness, but has no effect on cognitive performance in dehydrated healthy young volunteers. Appetite 37: 255–256.
    1. Edmonds CJ, Crombie R, Ballieux H, Gardner MR, Dawkins L (2013) Water consumption, not expectancies about water consumption, affects cognitive performance in adults. Appetite 60: 148–153.
    1. Benton D, Burgess N (2009) The effect of the consumption of water on the memory and attention of children. Appetite 53: 143–146.
    1. Booth P, Taylor B, Edmonds CJ (2012) Water supplementation improves visual attention and fine motor skills in schoolchildren. Education and Health 30: 75–79.
    1. Edmonds CJ, Burford D (2009) Should children drink more water?: the effects of drinking water on cognition in children. Appetite 52: 776–779.
    1. Edmonds CJ, Jeffes B (2009) Does having a drink help you think? 6-7-Year-old children show improvements in cognitive performance from baseline to test after having a drink of water. Appetite 53: 469–472.
    1. Le Bellego L, Jean C, Jimenez L, Magnani C, Tang W, et al. (2010) Understanding fluid consumption patterns to improve healthy hydration. Nutrition Today 47: 77–81.
    1. Vergne S (2012) Methodological aspects of fluid intake records and surveys. Nutrition Today 47: S7–S10.
    1. Perrier E, Demazieres A, Girard N, Pross N, Osbild D, et al. (2013) Circadian variation and responsiveness of hydration biomarkers to changes in daily water intake. Eur J Appl Physiol doi:.
    1. Boivin DB, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ, Duffy JF, Folkard S, et al. (1997) Complex interaction of the sleep-wake cycle and circadian phase modulates mood in healthy subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54: 145–152.
    1. Cornelissen G, Watson D, Mitsutake G, Fiser B, Siegelova J, et al. (2005) Mapping of circaseptan and circadian changes in mood. Scr Med (Brno) 78: 89–98.
    1. Rolls BJ, Wood RJ, Rolls ET, Lind H, Lind W, et al. (1980) Thirst following water deprivation in humans. Am J Physiol 239: R476–R482.
    1. Akerstedt T, Gillberg M (1990) Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 52: 29–37.
    1. Lees N, Lloyd-Williams M (1999) Assessing depression in palliative care patients using the visual analogue scale: a pilot study. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl) 8: 220–223.
    1. McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF (1981) Manual of the profile of mood state. San Diego: Educational and industrial testing service.
    1. Bond A, Lader M (1974) The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. British Journal of Medical Psychology 47: 211–218.
    1. Das RN, Grimmer-Somers KA (2012) Fluid intake and voiding; habits and health knowledge in a young healthy population. Research and Reports in Urology 4: 9–15.

Source: PubMed

3
Iratkozz fel