Pupillometry Assessment of Speech Recognition and Listening Experience in Adult Cochlear Implant Patients

Francesca Yoshie Russo, Michel Hoen, Chadlia Karoui, Thomas Demarcy, Marine Ardoint, Maria-Pia Tuset, Daniele De Seta, Olivier Sterkers, Ghizlène Lahlou, Isabelle Mosnier, Francesca Yoshie Russo, Michel Hoen, Chadlia Karoui, Thomas Demarcy, Marine Ardoint, Maria-Pia Tuset, Daniele De Seta, Olivier Sterkers, Ghizlène Lahlou, Isabelle Mosnier

Abstract

Objective: The aim of the present study was to investigate the pupillary response to word identification in cochlear implant (CI) patients. Authors hypothesized that when task difficulty (i.e., addition of background noise) increased, pupil dilation markers such as the peak dilation or the latency of the peak dilation would increase in CI users, as already observed in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired subjects.

Methods: Pupillometric measures in 10 CI patients were combined to standard speech recognition scores used to evaluate CI outcomes, namely, speech audiometry in quiet and in noise at +10 dB signal-to-noise ratio (SNR). The main outcome measures of pupillometry were mean pupil dilation, maximal pupil dilation, dilation latency, and mean dilation during return to baseline or retention interval. Subjective hearing quality was evaluated by means of one self-reported fatigue questionnaire, and the Speech, Spatial, and Qualities (SSQ) of Hearing scale.

Results: All pupil dilation data were transformed to percent change in event-related pupil dilation (ERPD, %). Analyses show that the peak amplitudes for both mean pupil dilation and maximal pupil dilation were higher during the speech-in-noise test. Mean peak dilation was measured at 3.47 ± 2.29% noise vs. 2.19 ± 2.46 in quiet and maximal peak value was detected at 9.17 ± 3.25% in noise vs. 8.72 ± 2.93% in quiet. Concerning the questionnaires, the mean pupil dilation during the retention interval was significantly correlated with the spatial subscale score of the SSQ Hearing scale [r(8) = -0.84, p = 0.0023], and with the global score [r(8) = -0.78, p = 0.0018].

Conclusion: The analysis of pupillometric traces, obtained during speech audiometry in quiet and in noise in CI users, provided interesting information about the different processes engaged in this task. Pupillometric measures could be indicative of listening difficulty, phoneme intelligibility, and were correlated with general hearing experience as evaluated by the SSQ of Hearing scale. These preliminary results show that pupillometry constitutes a promising tool to improve objective quantification of CI performance in clinical settings.

Keywords: cochlear implant; listening effort; pupil dilatation; pupillometery; speech in noise.

Copyright © 2020 Russo, Hoen, Karoui, Demarcy, Ardoint, Tuset, De Seta, Sterkers, Lahlou and Mosnier.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
General trial structure and timing of events.
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
Scatter-plot of individual speech audiometry scores obtained in quiet (Q) and in noise (N) percent correct identification scores for Lafon words (W) and phonemes (P), in red: mean and standard error of mean.
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
Effect of conditions. Grand-averaged (N = 10) pupillometry traces showing percent change in event-related pupil dilation (ERPD) for correctly recognized words, presented in Quiet (black line) or +10 dB speech-weighed noise background (gray dashed). Time is given relative to the onset of the noise in noisy background set at 0 s, words are presented at 3 s, and noise fades-out at 6 s.
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Bar-graphs representing percent change in event-related pupil dilation (ERPD) averaged over the three time-windows (Background, Peak, and Retention interval) for correct words presented in the Quiet (black) or Noise (gray) conditions. Error bars represent standard deviation of the mean.
FIGURE 5
FIGURE 5
Performance effect. Grand-averaged (N = 10) pupillometry traces showing percent change in event-related pupil dilation (ERPD) for correctly recognized words (black line) or incorrect trials (gray line), independently of the listening condition.
FIGURE 6
FIGURE 6
Grand-averaged (N = 10) pupillometry traces showing percent change in event-related pupil dilation (ERPD) analyzed across trials leading to 0, 1, 2, or 3 phonemes recognition.

References

    1. Alhanbali S., Dawes P., Lloyd S., Munro K. J. (2017). Self-Reported Listening-Related Effort and Fatigue in Hearing-Impaired Adults. Ear Hear. 38:e39–e48. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000361
    1. Alhanbali S., Dawes P., Lloyd S., Munro K. J. (2018). Hearing Handicap and Speech Recognition Correlate With Self-Reported Listening Effort and Fatigue. Ear Hear. 39 470–474. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000515
    1. Ambert-Dahan E., Routier S., Marot L., Bouccara D., Sterkers O., Ferrary E., et al. (2017). Cognitive Evaluation of Cochlear Implanted Adults Using CODEX and MoCA Screening Tests. Otol. Neurotol. 38:e282–e284. 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001464
    1. Antoniou M., Wong P. C. (2015). Poor phonetic perceivers are affected by cognitive load when resolving talker variability. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 138 571–574. 10.1121/1.4923362
    1. Beatty J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychol. Bull. 91, 276–292. 10.1037/0033-2909.91.2.276
    1. Beatty J., Lucero-Wagoner B. (2000). “The pupillary system,” in Handbook of psychophysiology, eds Cacioppo J. T., Tassinary L. G., Berntson G. G. (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; ), 142–162.
    1. Ben-Nun Y. (1986). The use of pupillometry in the study of on-line verbal processing: Evidence for depths of processing. Brain Lang. 28 1–11. 10.1016/0093-934x(86)90086-6
    1. Bonnard D., Lautissier S., Bosset-Audoit A., Coriat G., Beraha M., Maunoury A., et al. (2013). Comparison between Bilateral Cochlear Implants and Neurelec Digisonict SP Binaural Cochlear Implant: Speech Perception. Sound Localization and Patient Self-Assessment. Audiol. Neurotol. 18 171–183. 10.1159/000346933
    1. Davis M. H., Johnsrude I. S. (2007). Hearing speech sounds: top-down influences on the interface between audition and speech perception. Hear. Res. 229 132–147. 10.1016/j.heares.2007.01.014
    1. Deal J. A., Albert M. S., Arnold M., Bangdiwala S. I., Chisolm T., Davis S., et al. (2017). A randomized feasibility pilot trial of hearing treatment for reducing cognitive decline: Results from the Aging and Cognitive Health Evaluation in Elders Pilot Study. Alzheimers Dement 3 410–415. 10.1016/j.trci.2017.06.003
    1. Gatehouse S., Noble W. (2004). The Speech. Spatial and Qualities of Hearing Scale (SSQ). Int. J. Audiol. 43 85–99. 10.80/14992020400050014
    1. Hornsby B. W., Naylor G., Bess F. H. (2016). A Taxonomy of Fatigue Concepts and Their Relation to Hearing Loss. Ear Hear. 37 136S–144S. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000289
    1. Hughes K. C., Galvin K. L. (2013). Measuring listening effort expended by adolescents and young adults with unilateral or bilateral cochlear implants or normal hearing. Cochlear Implants Int. 14 121–129. 10.1179/1754762812Y.0000000009
    1. Just M. A., Carpenter P. A. (1993). The intensity dimension of thought: Pupillometric indices of sentence processing. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 47 310–339. 10.1037/h0078820
    1. Kahneman D. (1973). Attention and Effort. New Jersey, NJ: Englewood Cliffs.
    1. Kahneman D., Beatty J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science 154 1583–1585. 10.1126/science.154.3756.1583
    1. Koelewijn T., de Kluiver H., Shinn-Cunningham B. G., Zekveld A. A., Kramer S. E. (2015). The pupil response reveals increased listening effort when it is difficult to focus attention. Hear. Res. 323 81–90. 10.1016/j.heares.2015.02.004
    1. Koelewijn T., Shinn-Cunningham B. G., Zekveld A. A., Kramer S. E. (2014). The pupil response is sensitive to divided attention during speech processing. Hear. Res. 312 114–120. 10.1016/j.heares.2014.03.010
    1. Koelewijn T., Zekveld A. A., Festen J. M., Kramer S. E. (2012). Pupil dilation uncovers extra listening effort in the presence of a single-talker masker. Ear Hear. 33 291–300. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182310019
    1. Koelewijn T., Zekveld A. A., Lunner T., Kramer S. E. (2018). The effect of reward on listening effort as reflected by the pupil dilation response. Hear. Res. 367 106–112. 10.1016/j.heares.2018.07.011
    1. Kramer S. E., Kapteyn T. S., Festen J. M., Kuik D. J. (1997). Assessing aspects of auditory handicap by means of pupil dilatation. Audiology 36 155–164. 10.3109/00206099709071969
    1. Kramer S. E., Teunissen C., Zekveld A. A. (2016). Cortisol, chromogranin A, and pupillary responses evoked by speech recognition tasks in normally hearing and hard-of-hearing listeners: a pilot study. Ear Hear. 37 126S–135S. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000311
    1. Kuchinsky S. E., Ahlstrom J. B., Vaden K. I., Jr., Cute S. L., Humes L. E., Dubno J. R., et al. (2013). Pupil size varies with word listening and response selection difficulty in older adults with hearing loss. Psychophysiology 50 23–34. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01477.x
    1. Kuznetsova A., Brockhoff P., Christensen R. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. J. Stat. Soft. 82 1–26. 10.18637/jss.v082.i13
    1. Lin F. R. (2011). Hearing loss and cognition among older adults in the United States. J. Gerontol. Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66 1131–1136. 10.1093/gerona/glr115
    1. Lin F. R., Yaffe K., Xia J., Xue Q. L., Harris T. B., Purchase-Helzner E., et al. (2013). Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med. 173 293–299. 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1868
    1. Lin V. Y., Chung J., Callahan B. L., Smith L., Gritters N., Chen J. M., et al. (2017). Development of cognitive screening test for the severely hearing impaired: Hearing-impaired MoCA. Laryngoscope 127(Suppl. 1), S4–S11. 10.1002/lary.26590
    1. Marsella P., Scorpecci A., Cartocci G., Giannantonio S., Maglione A. G., Venuti I., et al. (2017). EEG activity as an objective measure of cognitive load during effortful listening: A study on pediatric subjects with bilateral, asymmetric sensorineural hearing loss. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinol. 99 1–7. 10.1016/j.ijporl.2017.05.006
    1. Mertens G., Punte A. K., Van de Heyning P. (2013). Self-assessment of hearing disabilities in cochlear implant users using the SSQ and the reduced SSQ5 version. Otol. Neurotol. 34 1622–1629. 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001083
    1. Moulin A., Pauzie A., Richard C. (2015). Validation of a French translation of the Speech. Spatial, and Qualities of Hearing Scale (SSQ) and comparison with other language versions. Int. J. Audiol. 54 889–898. 10.3109/14992027.2015.1054040
    1. Ng E. H., Rudner M., Lunner T., Pedersen M. S., Rönnberg J. (2013). Effects of noise and working memory capacity on memory processing of speech for hearing-aid users. Int. J. Audiol. 52 433–441. 10.3109/14992027.2013.776181
    1. Pals C., Sarampalis A., Başkent D. (2013). Listening effort with cochlear implant simulations. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56 1075–1084. 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0074)
    1. Pals C., Sarampalis A., van Rijn H., Başkent D. (2015). Validation of a simple response-time measure of listening effort. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 138 EL187–EL192. 10.1121/1.4929614
    1. Peelle J. E. (2018). Listening Effort: How the Cognitive Consequences of Acoustic Challenge Are Reflected in Brain and Behavior. Ear Hear. 39 204–214. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000494
    1. Pendlebury S. T., Klaus S. P., Mather M., de Brito M., Wharton R. M. (2015). Routine cognitive screening in older patients admitted to acute medicine: abbreviated mental test score (AMTS) and subjective memory complaint versus Montreal Cognitive Assessment and IQCODE. Age Ageing 44 1000–1005. 10.1093/ageing/afv134
    1. Pichora-Fuller M. K. (2016). How social factors may modulate auditory and cognitive functioning during listening. Ear Hear. 37 92S–100S. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000323
    1. Pichora-Fuller M. K., Kramer S. E., Eckert M. A., Edwards B., Hornsby B. W., Humes L. E., et al. (2016). Hearing Impairment and Cognitive Energy: The Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening (FUEL). Ear Hear. 37(Suppl. 1), 5s–27s. 10.1097/aud.0000000000000312
    1. Pichora-Fuller M. K., Mick P., Reed M. (2015). Hearing. Cognition, and Healthy Aging: Social and Public Health Implications of the Links between Age-Related Declines in Hearing and Cognition. Semin. Hear. 36 122–139. 10.1055/s-0035-1555116
    1. Ramakers G. G. J., Smulders Y. E., van Zon A., Van Zanten G. A., Grolman W., Stegeman I. (2017). Correlation between subjective and objective hearing tests after unilateral and bilateral cochlear implantation. BMC Ear Nose Throat Disord. 17:10. 10.1186/s12901-017-0043-y
    1. Steel M. M., Papsin B. C., Gordon K. A. (2015). Binaural fusion and listening effort in children who use bilateral cochlear implants: a psychoacoustic and pupillometric study. PLoS One 10:e0117611. 10.1371/journal.pone.0117611
    1. Urai A. E., Braun A., Donner T. H. (2017). Pupil-linked arousal is driven by decision uncertainty and alters serial choice bias. Nat. Commun. 8:14637. 10.1038/ncomms14637
    1. Wagner A., Pals C., de Blecourt C. M., Sarampalis A., Başkent D. (2016). Does Signal Degradation Affect Top-Down Processing of Speech? Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 894 297–306. 10.1007/978-3-319-25474-6_31
    1. Wagner A. E., Nagels L., Toffanin P., Opie J. M., Başkent D. (2019). Individual Variations in Effort: Assessing Pupillometry for the Hearing Impaired. Trends Hear. 23:2331216519845596. 10.1177/2331216519845596
    1. Wagner A. E., Toffanin P., Başkent D. (2016). The Timing and Effort of Lexical Access in Natural and Degraded Speech. Front. Psychol. 7:398. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00398
    1. Wang Y., Naylor G., Kramer S. E., Zekveld A. A., Wendt D., Ohlenforst B., et al. (2018). Relations Between Self-Reported Daily-Life Fatigue, Hearing Status, and Pupil Dilation During a Speech Perception in Noise Task. Ear Hear. 39 573–582. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000512
    1. Wang Y., Zekveld A. A., Naylor G., Ohlenforst B., Jansma E. P., Lorens A., et al. (2016). Parasympathetic Nervous System Dysfunction, as Identified by Pupil Light Reflex, and Its Possible Connection to Hearing Impairment. PLoS One 11:e0153566. 10.1371/journal.pone.0153566
    1. Weisz N., Obleser J. (2014). Synchronisation signatures in the listening brain: a perspective from non-invasive neuroelectrophysiology. Hear Res. 307 16–28. 10.1016/j.heares.2013.07.009
    1. Winn M. (2016). Rapid Release From Listening Effort Resulting From Semantic Context, and Effects of Spectral Degradation and Cochlear Implants. Trends Hear. 20:2331216516669723. 10.1177/2331216516669723
    1. Winn M. B., Edwards J. R., Litovsky R. Y. (2015). The Impact of Auditory Spectral Resolution on Listening Effort Revealed by Pupil Dilation. Ear Hear. 36 153–165e. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000145
    1. Winn M. B., Moore A. N. (2018). Pupillometry Reveals That Context Benefit in Speech Perception Can Be Disrupted by Later-Occurring Sounds. Especially in Listeners With Cochlear Implants. Trends Hear. 22:2331216518808962. 10.1177/2331216518808962
    1. Winn M. B., Wendt D., Koelewijn T., Kuchinsky S. E. (2018). Best practices and advice for using pupillometry to measure listening effort: an introduction for those Who Want to get started. Trends Hear. 22:2331216518800869. 10.1177/2331216518800869
    1. Winn M. B., Won J. H., Moon I. J. (2016). Assessment of spectral and temporal resolution in cochlear implant users using psychoacoustic discrimination and speech cue categorization. Ear Hear. 37:e377–e390. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000328
    1. Zekveld A. A., Festen J. M., Kramer S. E. (2013). Task difficulty differentially affects two measures of processing load: The pupil response during sentence processing and delayed cued recall of the sentences. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56 1156–1165. 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0058)
    1. Zekveld A. A., Koelewijn T., Kramer S. E. (2018). The Pupil Dilation Response to Auditory Stimuli: Current State of Knowledge. Trends Hear. 22:2331216518777174. 10.1177/2331216518777174
    1. Zekveld A. A., Kramer S. E., Festen J. M. (2010). Pupil response as an indication of effortful listening: the influence of sentence intelligibility. Ear Hear. 31 480–490. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181d4f251
    1. Zekveld A. A., Kramer S. E., Festen J. M. (2011). Cognitive load during speech perception in noise: the influence of age, hearing loss, and cognition on the pupil response. Ear Hear. 32 498–510. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31820512bb
    1. Zhao S., Bury G., Milne A., Chait M. (2019). Pupillometry as an Objective Measure of Sustained Attention in Young and Older Listeners. Trends Hear. 23:2331216519887815. 10.1177/2331216519887815

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnere