Video-game play induces plasticity in the visual system of adults with amblyopia

Roger W Li, Charlie Ngo, Jennie Nguyen, Dennis M Levi, Roger W Li, Charlie Ngo, Jennie Nguyen, Dennis M Levi

Abstract

Abnormal visual experience during a sensitive period of development disrupts neuronal circuitry in the visual cortex and results in abnormal spatial vision or amblyopia. Here we examined whether playing video games can induce plasticity in the visual system of adults with amblyopia. Specifically 20 adults with amblyopia (age 15-61 y; visual acuity: 20/25-20/480, with no manifest ocular disease or nystagmus) were recruited and allocated into three intervention groups: action videogame group (n = 10), non-action videogame group (n = 3), and crossover control group (n = 7). Our experiments show that playing video games (both action and non-action games) for a short period of time (40-80 h, 2 h/d) using the amblyopic eye results in a substantial improvement in a wide range of fundamental visual functions, from low-level to high-level, including visual acuity (33%), positional acuity (16%), spatial attention (37%), and stereopsis (54%). Using a cross-over experimental design (first 20 h: occlusion therapy, and the next 40 h: videogame therapy), we can conclude that the improvement cannot be explained simply by eye patching alone. We quantified the limits and the time course of visual plasticity induced by video-game experience. The recovery in visual acuity that we observed is at least 5-fold faster than would be expected from occlusion therapy in childhood amblyopia. We used positional noise and modelling to reveal the neural mechanisms underlying the visual improvements in terms of decreased spatial distortion (7%) and increased processing efficiency (33%). Our study had several limitations: small sample size, lack of randomization, and differences in numbers between groups. A large-scale randomized clinical study is needed to confirm the therapeutic value of video-game treatment in clinical situations. Nonetheless, taken as a pilot study, this work suggests that video-game play may provide important principles for treating amblyopia, and perhaps other cortical dysfunctions.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT01223716.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Consort flow diagrams.
Figure 1. Consort flow diagrams.
This research project was commenced in late 2004 and completed in early 2009. The first author (RWL) was responsible for conducting clinical procedures in screening patients and assigning participants to interventions. Participants were pseudo-randomly allocated into three intervention groups. The first 10 enrolled patients participated in the action videogame group (MOH), the subsequently enrolled three patients participated in the non-action videogame group (SIM), and then another seven patients were recruited in the crossover intervention group (phase 1: occlusion therapy; phase 2: video game therapy, “joypad” symbol  =  MOH or SIM). Note that the subject allocation was not based on the clinical characteristics of participants.
Figure 2. Improved visual acuity (VA) with…
Figure 2. Improved visual acuity (VA) with video-game experience.
(A) Action video game. Color coding is used throughout the figures to represent the type of amblyopia. Red, strabismic; green, anisometropic; Blue, mixed (strabismic & anisometropic); dark purple, mixed (strabismic & deprivation). Error bars: one s.e.m. (here and in all subsequent figures). (B) Non-action video game. In this experiment, participants were required to play a non-action video game (“chess” symbol: SIM) in the first 40 h and an action video game (“gun” symbol: MOH) in the second 40 h. Note that given the small sample size, the fitting curve is provided here for reference. (C) Control experiment. Another group of participants was required to first undertake occlusion therapy (OT, “patch” symbol) for 20 h, and then continue to the video-game phase (“joypad” symbol: MOH or SIM). Note that SB3 was not available to finish the complete course of video-game training. (D) Summary of acuity data. (Top left) A schematic logMAR letter chart. Each 0.1 logMAR represents 1 letter-line. Parentheses: Snellen acuity. (Top right) The visual acuity data from panels a–c are pooled together to calculate the mean data. (Bottom left) Percent improvement is replotted as a function of baseline visual acuity. Solid symbols: crowded acuity. Open symbols: uncrowded acuity. (Bottom right) Effect of video-game experience on visual crowding. Shaded area: decreased visual crowding.
Figure 3. Improved positional acuity with video-game…
Figure 3. Improved positional acuity with video-game experience.
(a) Position discrimination. The visual task was to pick the misaligned pair of Gabor patch groupings out of three choices (top, middle, or bottom) . Each grouping consisted of 8 Gabor patches. Positional noise to the Gabor patches was introduced by varying their vertical positions according to a Gaussian distribution function. (b) Percent improvement in positional acuity as a function of baseline positional acuity (zero noise). Each data point represents the mean improvement across different noise levels. (c) Effect of video-game experience on sampling efficiency. (d) Effect of video-game experience on internal noise. (e) Threshold versus noise (TvN) function. Three different neural mechanism signature profiles are illustrated. SB2: TvN function shifts downward (increase in efficiency). SA5: The knee point of TvN function shifts downward and to the left (decrease in internal noise). SS3: combination of both.
Figure 4. Improved spatial attention with video-game…
Figure 4. Improved spatial attention with video-game experience.
(a) Visual counting. A number (N = 1–10 dots) of black circular dots was presented for 200 ms against a gray background. The target stimulus was then followed by a checkerboard pattern for another 100 ms. Observers were asked to enumerate the number of dots as quickly and accurately as they could. No feedback was given. Note that the dot size was scaled with visual acuity, and therefore the dots displayed on the screen were very visible. (b) Counting threshold. Non-amblyopic eye (NAE) versus amblyopic eye (AE). (c) Percent improvement of counting threshold in the amblyopic eye after video-game intervention. SIM: n = 4 (dotted circles). MOH: n = 10. (d–e) Subgroup analysis—Undercounting. (d) Number of dots reported as a function of number of dots displayed. (e) Counting threshold calculation. An arrow indicates an increase in counting threshold. (f) Response latency as a function of number of dots.
Figure 5. Improved stereoacuity in anisometropic amblyopia…
Figure 5. Improved stereoacuity in anisometropic amblyopia with video-game experience.
(a) Stereoacuity as a function of video-game hours. The normal stereoacuity range is 20–40 arcsec. Dotted line: the lower measuring limit of the stereo test plates. Note: JS failed the test in the baseline session; her initial data point is thus arbitrarily set to 400 arcsec (the upper measuring limit of the test plates). (b) Stereoacuity data were replotted in terms of percent improvement.

References

    1. Friedman D. S, Repka M. X, Katz J, Giordano L, Ibironke J, et al. Prevalence of amblyopia and strabismus in white and African American children aged 6 through 71 months the Baltimore Pediatric Eye Disease Study. Ophthalmology. 2009;116:2128–2134 e2121–2122.
    1. Drover J. R, Kean P. G, Courage M. L, Adams R. J. Prevalence of amblyopia and other vision disorders in young Newfoundland and Labrador children. Can J Ophthalmol. 2008;43:89–94.
    1. Levi D. M, Polat U. Neural plasticity in adults with amblyopia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93:6830–6834.
    1. Li R. W, Levi D. M. Characterizing the mechanisms of improvement for position discrimination in adult amblyopia. J Vis. 2004;6:476–487.
    1. Polat U, Ma-Naim T, Belkin M, Sagi D. Improving vision in adult amblyopia by perceptual learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:6692–6697.
    1. Li R. W, Young K. G, Hoenig P, Levi D. M. Perceptual learning improves visual perception in juvenile amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46:3161–3168.
    1. Zhou Y, Huang C, Xu P, Tao L, Qiu Z, et al. Perceptual learning improves contrast sensitivity and visual acuity in adults with anisometropic amblyopia. Vision Res. 2006;46:739–750.
    1. Chung S. T. L, Li R. W, Levi D. M. Identification of contrast-defined letters in adults with amblyopia benefits from perceptual learning. Vision Res. 2006;46:3853–3861.
    1. Li R. W, Provost A, Levi D. M. Extended perceptual learning results in substantial recovery of both positional acuity and visual acuity in juvenile amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:5046–5051.
    1. Chung S. T, Li R. W, Levi D. M. Learning to identify near-threshold luminance-defined and contrast-defined letters in observers with amblyopia. Vision Res. 2008;48:2739–2750.
    1. Li R. W, Klein S. A, Levi D. M. Prolonged perceptual learning of positional acuity in adult amblyopia: perceptual template retuning dynamics. J Neurosci. 2008;28:14223–14229.
    1. Polat U, Ma-Naim T, Spierer A. Treatment of children with amblyopia by perceptual learning. Vision Res. 2009;49:2599–2603.
    1. Sale A, Maya Vetencourt J. F, Medini P, Cenni M. C, Baroncelli L, et al. Environmental enrichment in adulthood promotes amblyopia recovery through a reduction of intracortical inhibition. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:679–681.
    1. He H. Y, Ray B, Dennis K, Quinlan E. M. Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1134–1136.
    1. Morishita H, Hensch T. K. Critical period revisited: impact on vision. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18:101–107.
    1. Buckley D, Codina C, Bhardwaj P, Pascalis O. Action video game players and deaf observers have larger Goldmann visual fields. Vision Res. 2010;50:548–556.
    1. Li R, Polat U, Makous W, Bavelier D. Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:549–551.
    1. Green C. S, Bavelier D. Action-video-game experience alters the spatial resolution of vision. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:88–94.
    1. Green C. S, Bavelier D. Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature. 2003;423:534–537.
    1. Levi D. M. Crowding-an essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vision Res. 2008;48:635–654.
    1. Stewart C. E, Moseley M. J, Stephens D. A, Fielder A. R. Treatment dose-response in amblyopia therapy: the Monitored Occlusion Treatment of Amblyopia Study (MOTAS). Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004;45:3048–3054.
    1. Chen P. L, Chen J. T, Fu J. J, Chien K. H, Lu D. W. A pilot study of anisometropic amblyopia improved in adults and children by perceptual learning: an alternative treatment to patching. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2008;28:422–428.
    1. Li R. W, Levi D. M, Klein S. A. Perceptual learning improves efficiency by re-tuning the “template” for position discrimination. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:178–183.
    1. Sharma V, Levi D. M, Klein S. A. Undercounting features and missing features: evidence for a high-level deficit in strabismic amblyopia. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:496–501.
    1. Burr D, Ross J. A visual sense of number. Curr Biol. 2008;18:425–428.
    1. Bonneh Y. S, Sagi D, Polat U. Spatial and temporal crowding in amblyopia. Vision Res. 2007;47:1950–1962.
    1. Huang C. B, Zhou Y, Lu Z. L. Broad bandwidth of perceptual learning in the visual system of adults with anisometropic amblyopia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:4068–4073.
    1. Levi D. M, Li R. W. Improving the performance of the amblyopic visual system. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364:399–407.
    1. Levi D. M, Li R. W. Perceptual learning as a potential treatment for amblyopia: a mini-review. Vision Res. 2009;49:2535–2549.
    1. Polat U. Making perceptual learning practical to improve visual functions. Vision Res. 2009;49:2566–2573.
    1. Cordes F. C, Harrington D. O. Toxic amblyopia due to tobacco and alcohol; treatment with vasodilators: report of eight cases. Arch Ophthal. 1935;13:435–444.
    1. Malik S. R, Singh P, Goel B. K. Long-term follow-up of red-filter treatment of amblyopia. Br J Ophthalmol. 1972;56:613–616.
    1. Metzler U, Ham O, Flores V, Claramunt M, Sepulveda C, et al. Blue filter amblyopia treatment protocol for strabismic amblyopia: a prospective comparative study of 50 cases. Binocul Vis Strabismus Q. 1998;13:241–248.
    1. Campbell F. W, Hess R. F, Watson P. G, Banks R. Preliminary results of a physiologically based treatment of amblyopia. Br J Ophthalmol. 1978;62:748–755.
    1. Galewski R. Electroacupuncture: an effective treatment of some visual disorders. Acupunct Med. 1999;17:42–48.
    1. Thompson B, Mansouri B, Koski L, Hess R. F. Brain plasticity in the adult: modulation of function in amblyopia with rTMS. Curr Biol. 2008;18:1067–1071.
    1. Maya Vetencourt J. F, Sale A, Viegi A, Baroncelli L, De Pasquale R, et al. The antidepressant fluoxetine restores plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science. 2008;320:385–388.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnere