Sharpening coarse-to-fine stereo vision by perceptual learning: asymmetric transfer across the spatial frequency spectrum

Roger W Li, Truyet T Tran, Ashley P Craven, Tsz-Wing Leung, Sandy W Chat, Dennis M Levi, Roger W Li, Truyet T Tran, Ashley P Craven, Tsz-Wing Leung, Sandy W Chat, Dennis M Levi

Abstract

Neurons in the early visual cortex are finely tuned to different low-level visual features, forming a multi-channel system analysing the visual image formed on the retina in a parallel manner. However, little is known about the potential 'cross-talk' among these channels. Here, we systematically investigated whether stereoacuity, over a large range of target spatial frequencies, can be enhanced by perceptual learning. Using narrow-band visual stimuli, we found that practice with coarse (low spatial frequency) targets substantially improves performance, and that the improvement spreads from coarse to fine (high spatial frequency) three-dimensional perception, generalizing broadly across untrained spatial frequencies and orientations. Notably, we observed an asymmetric transfer of learning across the spatial frequency spectrum. The bandwidth of transfer was broader when training was at a high spatial frequency than at a low spatial frequency. Stereoacuity training is most beneficial when trained with fine targets. This broad transfer of stereoacuity learning contrasts with the highly specific learning reported for other basic visual functions. We also revealed strategies to boost learning outcomes 'beyond-the-plateau'. Our investigations contribute to understanding the functional properties of the network subserving stereovision. The ability to generalize may provide a key principle for restoring impaired binocular vision in clinical situations.

Keywords: generalization; specificity; stereopsis; vision enhancement; visual plasticity.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Stereo stimulus. (a) The stereogram consisted of two slightly different pictures—one to each eye. At the centre each square was a target Gabor patch surrounded by four reference Gabor patches. To eliminate any possible monocular cues, the vertical and horizontal coordinates of each Gabor patch and also the patch features, the carrier phase, were randomly jittered according to a uniform distribution. A custom-built mirror stereoscope was used to view the stereo pairs, so that the left eye would see the left square and the right eye would see the right one. Binocular disparity was generated by shifting the two target Gabor patches, one on each side, horizontally in opposite directions (uncrossed disparity, both shifted temporally; crossed disparity, both shifted nasally). (b) Binocular fusion of the two monocular images creates a cyclopean image. The visual task was to determine the stereoscopic depth of the target Gabor (in front/behind) relative to the four adjacent references. This schematic diagram illustrates crossed disparity—the target Gabor patch appeared in front of the reference patches.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Experiment 1. Perceptual learning of stereoacuity: specificity for carrier orientation and spatial frequency. (a) The training protocol consisted of three training stages: stage 1, V5 (vertical carrier: 5 cpd); stage 2, H5 (horizontal carrier: 5 cpd); stage 3, V10 (vertical carrier: 10 cpd). A 3-parameter exponential function was used to quantify the learning profile. Mean thresholds (n=10) for each of the three stimulus configurations were measured before and after each training stage. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean unless stated otherwise. (b) The pre- and post-training threshold data of individual observers (n=10) are illustrated in the nine figure panels: 1st row, s15 versus s1; 2nd row, s29 versus s15; 3rd row, s43 versus s29. Arrows indicate the sequence of direct training from stage 1 to 3. White panel area denotes statistically significant. Grey panel area, not statistically significant. Note that the abscissa label for each row is displayed at the right-hand end of all the panels highlighted in dark blue.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Experiment 2. Bandwidth of generalization across the spatial frequency spectrum. (a) Mean stereoacuity as a function of session. In stage 1, group LH (first row, n=10) was trained with V1 stimuli and group HL (second row, n=11) was trained with V20 stimuli. In stage 2, observers crossed over and trained at the untrained spatial frequency: group LH, V20; group HL, V1. A 3-parameter exponential function was used to quantify the learning profile. (b) Mean stereoacuity across spatial frequencies. (c) Per cent improvement in mean stereoacuity (I) as a function of spatial frequency (f). A 3-parameter Gaussian function, I=It×e−(1/2)(fft/σ)2, was used to quantify the generalization of stereoacuity learning across spatial frequencies, where It is the per cent improvement occurring at the trained spatial frequency (ft) and σ denotes standard deviation.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Enhancing coarse-to-fine stereoacuity with perceptual learning. Mean per cent improvement in stereoacuity resulting from direct training as a function of spatial frequency (stage 1 of the two experiments; V5 from the first experiment; V1 and V20 from the second experiment). There was no significant difference in per cent improvement among the three frequency groups (ANOVA: F=0.795, p=0.461).

References

    1. Westheimer G. 1979. The spatial sense of the eye. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 18, 893–912.
    1. Roe AW, Parker AJ, Born RT, DeAngelis GC. 2007. Disparity channels in early vision. J. Neurosci. 27, 11 820–11 831. ()
    1. Anzai A, DeAngelis GC. 2010. Neural computations underlying depth perception. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 367–375. ()
    1. Nakatsuka C, Zhang B, Watanabe I, Zheng J, Bi H, Ganz L, Smith EL, Harwerth RS, Chino YM. 2007. Effects of perceptual learning on local stereopsis and neuronal responses of V1 and V2 in prism-reared monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 2612–2626. ()
    1. Chowdhury SA, DeAngelis GC. 2008. Fine discrimination training alters the causal contribution of macaque area MT to depth perception. Neuron 60, 367–377. ()
    1. Fendick M, Westheimer G. 1983. Effects of practice and the separation of test targets on foveal and peripheral stereoacuity. Vision Res. 23, 145–150. ()
    1. Ding J, Levi DM. 2011. Recovery of stereopsis through perceptual learning in human adults with abnormal binocular vision. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, E733–E741. ()
    1. Xi J, Jia W-L, Feng L-X, Lu Z-L, Huang C-B. 2014. Perceptual learning improves stereoacuity in amblyopiaperceptual learning and restoration of stereoacuity. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 55, 2384–2391. ()
    1. Fiorentini A, Berardi N. 1980. Perceptual learning specific for orientation and spatial frequency. Nature 287, 43–44. ()
    1. Ball K, Sekular R. 1982. A specific and enduring improvement in visual motion discrimination. Science 218, 697–698. ()
    1. Karni A, Sagi D. 1991. Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 88, 4966–4970. ()
    1. Schoups AA, Vogels R, Orban GA. 1995. Human perceptual learning in identifying the oblique orientation: retinotopy, orientation specificity and monocularity. J. Physiol. 483, 797–810. ()
    1. Ahissar M, Hochstein S. 1997. Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning. Nature 387, 401–406. ()
    1. Fahle M. 1997. Specificity of learning curvature, orientation, and vernier discriminations. Vision Res. 37, 1885–1895. ()
    1. Ramachandran VS, Braddick O. 1973. Orientation-specific learning in stereopsis. Perception 2, 371–376. ()
    1. Ramachandran VS. 1976. Learning-like phenomena in stereopsis. Nature 262, 382–384. ()
    1. Long NR. 1982. Transfer of learning in transformed random-dot stereostimuli. Perception 11, 409–414. ()
    1. O’Toole AJ, Kersten DJ. 1992. Learning to see random-dot stereograms. Perception 21, 227–243. ()
    1. Kumar T, Glaser DA. 1993. Initial performance, learning and observer variability for hyperacuity tasks. Vision Res. 33, 2287–2230. ()
    1. Fahle M, Edelman S, Poggio T. 1995. Fast perception learning in hyperacuity. Vision Res. 35, 3003–3013. ()
    1. Sowden P, Davies I, Rose D, Kaye M. 1996. Perceptual learning of stereoacuity. Perception 25, 1043–1052. ()
    1. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. 1968. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J. Physiol. 195, 215–243. ()
    1. DeAngelis GC. 2000. Seeing in three dimensions: the neurophysiology of stereopsis. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 80–90. ()
    1. Menz MD, Freeman RD. 2003. Stereoscopic depth processing in the visual cortex: a coarse-to-fine mechanism. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 59–65. ()
    1. Li RW, Levi DM, Klein SA. 2004. Perceptual learning improves efficiency by re-tuning the ‘template’ for position discrimination. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 178–183. ()
    1. Blakemore C, Campbell FW. 1969. On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images. J. Physiol. 203, 237–260. ()
    1. Wilson HR. 1978. Quantitative prediction of line spread function measurements: implications for channel bandwidths. Vision Res. 18, 493–496. ()
    1. De Valois RL, Albrecht DG, Thorell LG. 1982. Spatial frequency selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex. Vision Res. 22, 545–559. ()
    1. Serrano-Pedraza I, Grady JP, Read JC. 2012. Spatial frequency bandwidth of surround suppression tuning curves. J. Vis. 12, e24 ()
    1. Huang CB, Zhou Y, Lu ZL. 2008. Broad bandwidth of perceptual learning in the visual system of adults with anisometropic amblyopia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 4068–4073. ()
    1. Fahle M. 2005. Perceptual learning: specificity versus generalization. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 154–160. ()
    1. Hung SC, Seitz AR. 2014. Prolonged training at threshold promotes robust retinotopic specificity in perceptual learning. J. Neurosci. 34, 8423– 8431. ()
    1. Beard BL, Levi DM, Reich LN. 1995. Perceptual learning in parafoveal vision. Vision Res. 35, 1679–1690. ()
    1. Gantz L, Patel SS, Chung ST, Harwerth RS. 2007. Mechanisms of perceptual learning of depth discrimination in random dot stereograms. Vision Res. 47, 2170–2178. ()
    1. Gantz L, Bedell HE. 2010. Transfer of perceptual learning of depth discrimination between local and global stereograms. Vision Res. 50, 1891–1899. ()
    1. Schmitt C, Kromeier M, Bach M, Kommerell G. 2002. Interindividual variability of learning in stereoacuity. Graefes Arch. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 240, 704–709. ()
    1. Erkelens CJ, Collewijn H. 1985. Eye movements and stereopsis during dichoptic viewing of moving random-dot stereograms. Vision Res. 25, 1689–1700. ()
    1. Watanabe T, Sasaki Y. 2015. Perceptual learning: toward a comprehensive theory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 197–221. ()
    1. Levi DM, Li RW. 2009. Improving the performance of the amblyopic visual system. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 399–407. ()
    1. Li RW, Ngo C, Nguyen J, Levi DM. 2011. Video-game play induces plasticity in the visual system of adults with amblyopia. PLoS Biol. 9, e1001135 ()
    1. Gori S, Facoetti A. 2014. Perceptual learning as a possible new approach for remediation and prevention of developmental dyslexia. Vision Res. 99, 78–87. ()
    1. Selvander M, Åsman P. 2011. Stereoacuity and intraocular surgical skill: effect of stereoacuity level on virtual reality intraocular surgical performance. J. Refract. Surg. 37, 2188–2193. ()
    1. DeLoss DJ, Watanabe T, Andersen GJ. 2014. Optimization of perceptual learning: effects of task difficulty and external noise in older adults. Vision Res. 99, 37–45. ()
    1. Jeter PE, Dosher BA, Petrov A, Lu Z-L. 2009. Task precision at transfer determines specificity of perceptual learning. J. Vis. 9, e1 ()
    1. Xiao LQ, Zhang JY, Wang R, Klein SA, Levi DM, Yu C. 2008. Complete transfer of perceptual learning across retinal locations enabled by double training. Curr. Biol. 18, 1922–1926. ()
    1. Zhang JY, Zhang GL, Xiao LQ, Klein SA, Levi DM, Yu C. 2010. Rule-based learning explains visual perceptual learning and its specificity and transfer. J. Neurosci. 30, 12 323–12 328. ()

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnere