The hippocampus and inferential reasoning: building memories to navigate future decisions

Dagmar Zeithamova, Margaret L Schlichting, Alison R Preston, Dagmar Zeithamova, Margaret L Schlichting, Alison R Preston

Abstract

A critical aspect of inferential reasoning is the ability to form relationships between items or events that were not experienced together. This review considers different perspectives on the role of the hippocampus in successful inferential reasoning during both memory encoding and retrieval. Intuitively, inference can be thought of as a logical process by which elements of individual existing memories are retrieved and recombined to answer novel questions. Such flexible retrieval is sub-served by the hippocampus and is thought to require specialized hippocampal encoding mechanisms that discretely code events such that event elements are individually accessible from memory. In addition to retrieval-based inference, recent research has also focused on hippocampal processes that support the combination of information acquired across multiple experiences during encoding. This mechanism suggests that by recalling past events during new experiences, connections can be created between newly formed and existing memories. Such hippocampally mediated memory integration would thus underlie the formation of networks of related memories that extend beyond direct experience to anticipate future judgments about the relationships between items and events. We also discuss integrative encoding in the context of emerging evidence linking the hippocampus to the formation of schemas as well as prospective theories of hippocampal function that suggest memories are actively constructed to anticipate future decisions and actions.

Keywords: encoding; flexibility; hippocampus; inference; integration; memory; retrieval.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Inference tasks. (A) Transitive inference task with six elements. A set of overlapping training pairs forms an ordered hierarchy of relationships. Participants learn each individual training pair via feedback-based learning (e.g., A > B) and are then tested on novel inference and novel non-inference judgments. Items in inferential probe trials may be separated by one element in the hierarchy (e.g., B ? D, indicated as 1°) or two elements (e.g., B ? E, indicated as 2°). Novel non-inferential probes test knowledge of the relationship between the end items of the hierarchy (A ? F). (B) Acquired equivalence task. In stage one of training, participants are trained via feedback to associate two faces (F1 and F2) with a particular scene (S1). In stage two, participants learn to select a second scene (S2) when cued with one of the faces (F1). Inference is then measured as the proportion of trials on which participants choose S2 when cued with F2. The schematic depicts trained stimulus–response relationships (solid black arrows) and inferential relationships (dashed black arrows). (C) Associative inference task. Participants learn an overlapping set of associations (here, face–house associations), in which two stimuli (a man and a woman) are associated with a common third item (a house). Novel inference trials evaluate knowledge for the indirect relationship between items (who lives together in the same house).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Lesion and neuropsychological studies assessing the critical role of hippocampus in inference.(A) Associative inference task. Left panel: mean number of errors to criterion on training of two sets of overlapping associations (e.g., AB, BC). Right panel: Inference performance as measured by a preference index for indirectly related item (e.g., selecting C when cued with A). White bars denote sham operated control rats; blue bars denote hippocampally lesioned rats. Hippocampally lesioned rats learn individual relationships between item pairs at a rate similar to control rats, but fail on the inference test. Adapted by permission from Bunsey and Eichenbaum (1996), copyright 1996 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. (B) Transitive inference task. Rats with lesions disconnecting the hippocampus from its subcortical (fornix, dark blue bars) or cortical (entorhinal/perirhinal, light blue bars) target structures performed similarly to sham operated control rats (white bars) on trained associations (BC, CD) and novel non-inferential probe trials (AE). However, lesioned rats were severely impaired on inferential probe trials (BD). Adapted by permission from Dusek and Eichenbaum (1997), copyright 1997 National Academy of Sciences, USA. (C) Mean performance of control participants (white bars), patients with hippocampal atrophy (blue bars) or Parkinson's disease patients (orange bars) in an acquired equivalence task. Patients with hippocampal atrophy reached a criterion level of performance at a rate similar to control participants. In contrast, Parkinson's disease patients required more extensive training. However, at test, hippocampal patients were severely impaired on inferential probe trials relative to both control participants and patients with Parkinson's disease. Adapted by permission from Myers et al. (2003), copyright 2003 MIT Press. (D) Post-training hippocampal lesions (blue bars) impaired transitive inference judgments (BD) in mice, but enhanced performance on novel non-inferential probe trials (AE) involving the end items of the hierarchy relative to sham operated animals (white bars). Adapted by permission from DeVito et al. (2010a), copyright 2009 Wiley-Liss, INC.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Symbolic depiction of encoding and retrieval strategies that may support inference.(A) Retrieval-based inference through recall and recombination of individual memories. When encountering a novel inferential probe (e.g., AC), the individual elements may trigger hippocampal pattern completion mechanisms, leading to the retrieval of the previously encountered overlapping associations (AB, BC) that can be then recombined to answer novel questions. In this example, when having to select which of the two men lives with the woman, one can recall that the woman lives in the red house, and that the man on the left also lives in the red house. Therefore, the woman lives with the man on the left. (B) Integration of overlapping events during encoding. When encountering an event that overlaps with prior experience (e.g., experiencing BC after encountering AB), the overlapping element (B) may trigger hippocampal pattern completion, reactivating the prior memory. The current experience may then be encoded in the context of the reactivated memory to form an integrated (A-B-C) representation that combines elements from both events. In this example, the prior memory for the woman living in the red house may be reactivated when learning about the man living in the same house. The current and reactivated experiences can then be combined to form a novel association that the man and the woman live together.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Hippocampal retrieval activation during inferential reasoning tasks.(A) Bilateral anterior hippocampus demonstrated selective activation during novel inferential probe trials (AC) at retrieval in an associative inference task. In contrast, a posterior region of the hippocampus demonstrated equivalent activation during associative retrieval of overlapping trained associations (AB, BC), non-overlapping trained associations (DE), and inferential probe trials. Adapted by permission from Preston et al. (2004), copyright 2004 Wiley-Liss, INC. (B) During transitive inference, left hippocampal activation demonstrated greater retrieval activation for novel inference trials relative to non-inference trials, while right hippocampus showed increased activation during inference trials in which items were separated by one element in the hierarchy (1°) compared with items separated by two hierarchical elements (2°). Adapted by permission from Zalesak and Heckers (2009), copyright 2009 Elsevier.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Hippocampal encoding activation during inferential reasoning tasks.(A) Left hippocampal activation increased across training block for inner pairs in the transitive hierarchy (B > C) relative to outer pairs (A > B), but only for those participants who were successful on the inferential test. Adapted by permission from Greene et al. (2006), copyright 2006 MIT Press. (B) In an associative inference task, right hippocampal activation during encoding of overlapping associations (BC) was greater for trials in which the corresponding inference judgment (AC) was later correct relative to trials on which the inference judgment was later incorrect. Hippocampal activation during initially acquired associations (AB) was not related to subsequent inferential performance. Adapted from Zeithamova and Preston (2010). (C) Activation in left and right hippocampus during the training phase of an acquired equivalence task was correlated with individual differences in inference performance. Specifically, increases in bilateral hippocampal activation from the early to late portion of the training phase were associated with superior performance on inferential probe trials. Adapted by permission from Shohamy and Wagner (2008), copyright 2008 Elsevier.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Schematic depiction of alternative accounts of hippocampal representation in an associative inference task. Representations of overlapping events (AB, BC) are shown using a simplified two-layer architecture. The bottom layer contains units for each event element; the top layer contains hypothesized patterns of hippocampal representation. (A) Single integrated representation for overlapping events. According to this hypothesized structure, new, overlapping event elements (C) are encoded into an existing, reactivated memory (AB) to form a single composite representation for the two related associations. (B) Pattern separated representations of individual events. In this view, a new event (BC) with partial overlap to a previous memory (AB) would recruit a distinct hippocampal representation that preserves the details of each individual experience. Links between the common element (B) and each of the individual experiences could be used to mediate inference at encoding or retrieval. (C) Relational representation of overlapping events. In this framework, separate representations are maintained for overlapping events (AB, BC) and direct links between those events (at the level of the hippocampus) code their relationship to one another.

References

    1. Acuna B. D., Eliassen J. C., Donoghue J. P., Sanes J. N. (2002). Frontal and parietal lobe activation during transitive inference in humans. Cereb. Cortex 12, 1312–1321 10.1093/cercor/12.12.1312
    1. Addis D. R., Schacter D. L. (2008). Constructive episodic simulation: temporal distance and detail of past and future events modulate hippocampal engagement. Hippocampus 18, 227–237 10.1002/hipo.20405
    1. Addis D. R., Schacter D. L. (2011). The hippocampus and imagining the future: where do we stand? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:173 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00173
    1. Addis D. R., Wong A. T., Schacter D. L. (2007). Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45, 1363–1377 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016
    1. Bakker A., Kirwan C. B., Miller M., Stark C. E. (2008). Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Science 319, 1640–1642 10.1126/science.1152882
    1. Barense M. D., Bussey T. J., Lee A. C., Rogers T. T., Davies R. R., Saksida L. M., Murray E. A., Graham K. S. (2005). Functional specialization in the human medial temporal lobe. J. Neurosci. 25, 10239–10246 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2704-05.2005
    1. Barense M. D., Gaffan D., Graham K. S. (2007). The human medial temporal lobe processes online representations of complex objects. Neuropsychologia 45, 2963–2974 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.023
    1. Bartlett F. C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press
    1. Buckmaster C. A., Eichenbaum H., Amaral D. G., Suzuki W. A., Rapp P. R. (2004). Entorhinal cortex lesions disrupt the relational organization of memory in monkeys. J. Neurosci. 24, 9811–9825 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1532-04.2004
    1. Buckner R. L. (2010). The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 27–48, C21–C28. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163508
    1. Bunsey M., Eichenbaum H. (1996). Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans. Nature 379, 255–257 10.1038/379255a0
    1. Bussey T. J., Saksida L. M. (2007). Memory, perception, and the ventral visual-perirhinal-hippocampal stream: thinking outside of the boxes. Hippocampus 17, 898–908 10.1002/hipo.20320
    1. Christoff K., Prabhakaran V., Dorfman J., Zhao Z., Kroger J. K., Holyoak K. J., Gabrieli J. D. (2001). Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage 14, 1136–1149 10.1006/nimg.2001.0922
    1. Chun M. M., Phelps E. A. (1999). Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 844–847 10.1038/12222
    1. Cohen N. J., Eichenbaum H. E. (1993). Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal System. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press
    1. Coutureau E., Killcross A. S., Good M., Marshall V. J., Ward-Robinson J., Honey R. C. (2002). Acquired equivalence and distinctiveness of cues: II. Neural manipulations and their implications. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 28, 388–396 10.1037/0097-7403.28.4.388
    1. Daselaar S. M., Fleck M. S., Prince S. E., Cabeza R. (2006). The medial temporal lobe distinguishes old from new independently of consciousness. J. Neurosci. 26, 5835–5839 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0258-06.2006
    1. DeVito L. M., Kanter B. R., Eichenbaum H. (2010a). The hippocampus contributes to memory expression during transitive inference in mice. Hippocampus 20, 208–217 10.1002/hipo.20610
    1. DeVito L. M., Lykken C., Kanter B. R., Eichenbaum H. (2010b). Prefrontal cortex: role in acquisition of overlapping associations and transitive inference. Learn. Mem. 17, 161–167 10.1101/lm.1685710
    1. Diekelmann S., Born J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 114–126 10.1038/nrn2762
    1. Dusek J. A., Eichenbaum H. (1997). The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus relations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 7109–7114
    1. Eichenbaum H. (1999). The hippocampus and mechanisms of declarative memory. Behav. Brain Res. 103, 123–133 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00044-3
    1. Eichenbaum H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 41–50 10.1038/35036213
    1. Eichenbaum H., Cohen N. J. (2001). From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection: Memory Systems of the Brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press
    1. Eichenbaum H., Dudchenko P., Wood E., Shapiro M., Tanila H. (1999). The hippocampus, memory, and place cells: is it spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron 23, 209–226 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80773-4
    1. Eichenbaum H., Schoenbaum G., Young B., Bunsey M. (1996). Functional organization of the hippocampal memory system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 13500–13507
    1. Eichenbaum H., Yonelinas A. P., Ranganath C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 123–152 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328
    1. Ellenbogen J. M., Hu P. T., Payne J. D., Titone D., Walker M. P. (2007). Human relational memory requires time and sleep. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 7723–7728 10.1073/pnas.0700094104
    1. Frank M. J., Rudy J. W., Levy W. B., O'Reilly R. C. (2005). When logic fails: implicit transitive inference in humans. Mem. Cognit. 33, 742–750
    1. Frank M. J., Rudy J. W., O'Reilly R. C. (2003). Transitivity, flexibility, conjunctive representations, and the hippocampus. II. A computational analysis. Hippocampus 13, 341–354 10.1002/hipo.10084
    1. Gabrieli J. D. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49, 87–115 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.87
    1. Greene A. J., Gross W. L., Elsinger C. L., Rao S. M. (2006). An fMRI analysis of the human hippocampus: inference, context, and task awareness. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1156–1173 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1156
    1. Greene A. J., Gross W. L., Elsinger C. L., Rao S. M. (2007). Hippocampal differentiation without recognition: an fMRI analysis of the contextual cueing task. Learn. Mem. 14, 548–553 10.1101/lm.609807
    1. Gupta A. S., van der Meer M. A., Touretzky D. S., Redish A. D. (2010). Hippocampal replay is not a simple function of experience. Neuron 65, 695–705 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.034
    1. Hannula D. E., Greene A. J. (2012). The hippocampus reevaluated in automatic and unconscious expressions of memory: at a tipping point? Front. Hum. Neurosci. (in press).
    1. Hannula D. E., Ranganath C. (2008). Medial temporal lobe activity predicts successful relational memory binding. J. Neurosci. 28, 116–124 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3086-07.2008
    1. Hannula D. E., Tranel D., Cohen N. J. (2006). The long and the short of it: relational memory impairments in amnesia, even at short lags. J. Neurosci. 26, 8352–8359 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5222-05.2006
    1. Hassabis D., Kumaran D., Maguire E. A. (2007a). Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. J. Neurosci. 27, 14365–14374 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4549-07.2007
    1. Hassabis D., Kumaran D., Vann S. D., Maguire E. A. (2007b). Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 1726–1731 10.1073/pnas.0610561104
    1. Heckers S., Zalesak M., Weiss A. P., Ditman T., Titone D. (2004). Hippocampal activation during transitive inference in humans. Hippocampus 14, 153–162 10.1002/hipo.10189
    1. Hoffman K. L., McNaughton B. L. (2002). Coordinated reactivation of distributed memory traces in primate neocortex. Science 297, 2070–2073 10.1126/science.1073538
    1. Iordanova M. D., Killcross A. S., Honey R. C. (2007). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in acquired distinctiveness and equivalence of cues. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 1431–1436 10.1037/0735-7044.121.6.1431
    1. Ji D., Wilson M. A. (2007). Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 100–107 10.1038/nn1825
    1. Johnson A., Varberg Z., Benhardus J., Maahs A., Schrater P. (2012). The hippocampus and exploration: dynamically evolving behavior and neural representations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. (in press).
    1. Karlsson M. P., Frank L. M. (2009). Awake replay of remote experiences in the hippocampus. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 913–918 10.1038/nn.2344
    1. Klein S. B., Cosmides L., Tooby J., Chance S. (2002). Decisions and the evolution of memory: multiple systems, multiple functions. Psychol. Rev. 109, 306–329 10.1037/0033-295X.109.2.306
    1. Kuhl B. A., Shah A. T., Dubrow S., Wagner A. D. (2010). Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated reactivation during new learning. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 501–506 10.1038/nn.2498
    1. Kumaran D. (2012). The hippocampus, generalization, and structure learning: an empirical and theoretical perspective. Front. Hum. Neurosci. (in press).
    1. Kumaran D., McClelland J. L. (2010). Generalization through recurrence: an interactive model of the hippocampus. Soc. Neurosci. [Online].
    1. Kumaran D., Summerfield J. J., Hassabis D., Maguire E. A. (2009). Tracking the emergence of conceptual knowledge during human decision making. Neuron 63, 889–901 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.030
    1. Lee A. C., Buckley M. J., Pegman S. J., Spiers H., Scahill V. L., Gaffan D., Bussey T. J., Davies R. R., Kapur N., Hodges J. R., Graham K. S. (2005a). Specialization in the medial temporal lobe for processing of objects and scenes. Hippocampus 15, 782–797 10.1002/hipo.20101
    1. Lee A. C., Bussey T. J., Murray E. A., Saksida L. M., Epstein R. A., Kapur N., Hodges J. R., Graham K. S. (2005b). Perceptual deficits in amnesia: challenging the medial temporal lobe “mnemonic” view. Neuropsychologia 43, 1–11 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.017
    1. Lee A. C. H., Yeung L.-K., Barense M. D. (2012). The hippocampus and visual perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. (in press). 10.1002/hipo.20784
    1. Lewis P. A., Durrant S. J. (2011). Overlapping memory replay during sleep builds cognitive schemata. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 343–351 10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.004
    1. Marr D. (1971). Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 262, 23–81 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078
    1. McClelland J. L., McNaughton B. L., O'Reilly R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 102, 419–457 10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.419
    1. Moses S. N., Villate C., Ryan J. D. (2006). An investigation of learning strategy supporting transitive inference performance in humans compared to other species. Neuropsychologia 44, 1370–1387 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.004
    1. Myers C. E., Shohamy D., Gluck M. A., Grossman S., Kluger A., Ferris S., Golomb J., Schnirman G., Schwartz R. (2003). Dissociating hippocampal versus basal ganglia contributions to learning and transfer. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 185–193 10.1162/089892903321208123
    1. Nagode J. C., Pardo J. V. (2002). Human hippocampal activation during transitive inference. Neuroreport 13, 939–944
    1. O'Keefe J., Nadel L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. London: Clarendon
    1. O'Reilly R. C., Rudy J. W. (2001). Conjunctive representations in learning and memory: principles of cortical and hippocampal function. Psychol. Rev. 108, 311–345 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.311
    1. Olson I. R., Moore K. S., Stark M., Chatterjee A. (2006a). Visual working memory is impaired when the medial temporal lobe is damaged. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1087–1097 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1087
    1. Olson I. R., Page K., Moore K. S., Chatterjee A., Verfaellie M. (2006b). Working memory for conjunctions relies on the medial temporal lobe. J. Neurosci. 26, 4596–4601 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1923-05.2006
    1. Pavlov I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. London: Oxford University Press
    1. Preston A. R., Bornstein A. M., Hutchinson J. B., Gaare M. E., Glover G. H., Wagner A. D. (2010). High-resolution fMRI of content-sensitive subsequent memory responses in human medial temporal lobe. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 156–173 10.1162/jocn.2009.21195
    1. Preston A. R., Gabrieli J. D. (2008). Dissociation between explicit memory and configural memory in the human medial temporal lobe. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2192–2207 10.1093/cercor/bhm245
    1. Preston A. R., Shrager Y., Dudukovic N. M., Gabrieli J. D. (2004). Hippocampal contribution to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory. Hippocampus 14, 148–152 10.1002/hipo.20009
    1. Preston A. R., Wagner A. D. (2007). “The medial temporal lobe and memory,” in Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 2nd Edn, eds Kesner R. P., Martinez J. L. (Oxford: Elsevier; ), 305–337
    1. Ranganath C. (2010). Binding items and contexts: the cognitive neuroscience of episodic memory. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 131–137 10.1002/hipo.20852
    1. Ranganath C., D'Esposito M. (2001). Medial temporal lobe activity associated with active maintenance of novel information. Neuron 31, 865–873 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00411-1
    1. Robin N., Holyoak K. J. (1995). “Relational complexity and the functions of the prefrontal cortex,” in The Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga M. S. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; ), 987–997
    1. Ryan J. D., Althoff R. R., Whitlow S., Cohen N. J. (2000). Amnesia is a deficit in relational memory. Psychol. Sci. 11, 454–461
    1. Sara S. J. (2010). Reactivation, retrieval, replay and reconsolidation in and out of sleep: connecting the dots. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:185 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00185
    1. Schnyer D. M., Dobbins I. G., Nicholls L., Schacter D. L., Verfaellie M. (2006). Rapid response learning in amnesia: delineating associative learning components in repetition priming. Neuropsychologia 44, 140–149 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.027
    1. Shohamy D., Wagner A. D. (2008). Integrating memories in the human brain: hippocampal-midbrain encoding of overlapping events. Neuron 60, 378–389 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.023
    1. Singer A. C., Karlsson M. P., Nathe A. R., Carr M. F., Frank L. M. (2010). Experience-dependent development of coordinated hippocampal spatial activity representing the similarity of related locations. J. Neurosci. 30, 11586–11604 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0926-10.2010
    1. Squire L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol. Rev. 99, 195–231 10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.195
    1. Squire L. R., Stark C. E., Clark R. E. (2004). The medial temporal lobe. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 279–306
    1. Tolman E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 55, 189–208
    1. Tse D., Langston R. F., Kakeyama M., Bethus I., Spooner P. A., Wood E. R., Witter M. P., Morris R. G. (2007). Schemas and memory consolidation. Science 316, 76–82 10.1126/science.1135935
    1. Tse D., Takeuchi T., Kakeyama M., Kajii Y., Okuno H., Tohyama C., Bito H., Morris R. G. (2011). Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science 333, 891–895 10.1126/science.1205274
    1. van der Jeugd A., Goddyn H., Laeremans A., Arckens L., D'Hooge R., Verguts T. (2009). Hippocampal involvement in the acquisition of relational associations, but not in the expression of a transitive inference task in mice. Behav. Neurosci. 123, 109–114 10.1037/a0013990
    1. von Fersen L., Wynne C. D. L., Delius J. D., Staddon J. E. R. (1991). Transitive inference in pigeons. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 17, 334–341
    1. Voss J. L., Warren D. E., Gonsalves B. D., Federmeier K. D., Tranel D., Cohen N. J. (2011). Spontaneous revisitation during visual exploration as a link among strategic behavior, learning, and the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, E402–E409 10.1073/pnas.1100225108
    1. Wallenstein G. V., Eichenbaum H., Hasselmo M. E. (1998). The hippocampus as an associator of discontiguous events. Trends Neurosci. 21, 317–323 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01220-4
    1. Wendelken C., Bunge S. A. (2010). Transitive inference: distinct contributions of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 837–847 10.1162/jocn.2009.21226
    1. Wood E. R., Dudchenko P. A., Eichenbaum H. (1999). The global record of memory in hippocampal neuronal activity. Nature 397, 613–616 10.1038/17605
    1. Zalesak M., Heckers S. (2009). The role of the hippocampus in transitive inference. Psychiatry Res. 172, 24–30 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.09.008
    1. Zeineh M. M., Engel S. A., Thompson P. M., Bookheimer S. Y. (2003). Dynamics of the hippocampus during encoding and retrieval of face-name pairs. Science 299, 577–580 10.1126/science.1077775
    1. Zeithamova D., Preston A. R. (2010). Flexible memories: differential roles for medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex in cross-episode binding. J. Neurosci. 30, 14676–14684 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3250-10.2010

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnere