Networked collective intelligence improves dissemination of scientific information regarding smoking risks

Douglas Guilbeault, Damon Centola, Douglas Guilbeault, Damon Centola

Abstract

Despite substantial investments in public health campaigns, misunderstanding of health-related scientific information is pervasive. This is especially true in the case of tobacco use, where smokers have been found to systematically misperceive scientific information about the negative health effects of smoking, in some cases leading smokers to increase their pro-smoking bias. Here, we extend recent work on 'networked collective intelligence' by testing the hypothesis that allowing smokers and nonsmokers to collaboratively evaluate anti-smoking advertisements in online social networks can improve their ability to accurately assess the negative health effects of tobacco use. Using Amazon's Mechanical Turk, we conducted an online experiment where smokers and nonsmokers (N = 1600) were exposed to anti-smoking advertisements and asked to estimate the negative health effects of tobacco use, either on their own or in the presence of peer influence in a social network. Contrary to popular predictions, we find that both smokers and nonsmokers were surprisingly inaccurate at interpreting anti-smoking messages, and their errors persisted if they continued to interpret these messages on their own. However, smokers and nonsmokers significantly improved in their ability to accurately interpret anti-smoking messages by sharing their opinions in structured online social networks. Specifically, subjects in social networks reduced the error of their risk estimates by over 10 times more than subjects who revised solely based on individual reflection (p < 0.001, 10 experimental trials in total). These results suggest that social media networks may be used to activate social learning that improves the public's ability to accurately interpret vital public health information.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. A schematic representation of the…
Fig 1. A schematic representation of the experimental design.
800 unique smokers and 800 unique nonsmokers were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: (1) a control condition where they interpreted the anti-smoking messages on their own; (2) an anonymous social network consisting of equal numbers of smokers and nonsmokers, where subjects exchanged views with peers for whom they were given no identifying information; and (3) a social network consisting of equal numbers of smokers and nonsmokers, where subjects exchanged views with peers while being aware of each other’s smoking status as either ‘smoker’ or ‘nonsmoker’. Every condition, in every trial, consisted of 40 unique subjects. Net. w. Sm. Habits Revealed, network with smoking habits of peers revealed.
Fig 2. Anti-smoking warning label used as…
Fig 2. Anti-smoking warning label used as a stimulus in the experiment.
This warning label was produced by the U.S. Department of Health & Human Services (© U.S. HHS) in 2011. The question that we used to elicit subjects’ judgements concerning the health risks of tobacco use was taken from the World Health Organization’s report on the global tobacco epidemic, 2015.
Fig 3. Changes in estimation error across…
Fig 3. Changes in estimation error across experimental conditions.
Bars display the total change in estimation error from Round 1 to Round 3, averaged across all 10 experimental trials, where each trial provides one observation. All conditions are independent. The error bars show 95% confidence intervals. S, smoker; NS, nonsmoker; An., anonymous; Id., with the smoking status of contacts identified.
Fig 4. Changes in the average estimation…
Fig 4. Changes in the average estimation error of individuals across experimental conditions, split by the smoking status of subjects in each condition.
(A) The performance of smokers, averaged across all 10 trials, for all conditions. (B) The performance of nonsmokers, averaged across all 10 trials, for all conditions. Network conditions contained 20 smokers and 20 nonsmokers, whereas each control condition contained 40 smokers and 40 nonsmokers. The average estimation accuracy of smokers and nonsmokers in networks was measured separately by computing the average estimation accuracy by each subgroup, within each network, thus producing two group-level observations for each network and 20 in total for each network condition. All conditions are independent. The error bars show 95% confidence intervals. w. Smoking Id., with the smoking status of contacts identified.

References

    1. WHO (The World Health Organization). WHO report on the global tobacco epidemic 2015. 2015; Available from: .
    1. CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). Tobacco-Related Mortality. 2017; Available from: .
    1. Cappella J, Maloney E, Ophir Y, Brennan E. Interventions to Correct Misinformation about Tobacco Products. 2015;1:186–197.
    1. Chou W, Oh A, Klein W. Addressing Health-Related Misinformation on Social Media. JAMA. 2018;320: 2417–2418. 10.1001/jama.2018.16865
    1. Wakefield M, Flay B, Nitcher M, Giovino G. Effects of Anti-Smoking Advertising on Youth Smoking: A Review. Journal of Health Communication. 2003;8: 229–247. 10.1080/10810730305686
    1. Johnsen B, Thayer J, Laberg J, Asbjornsen A. Attentional bias in active smokers, abstinent smokers, and nonsmokers. Addictive Behaviors. 1997;22: 813–817 10.1016/s0306-4603(97)00010-5
    1. Cummings KM, Hyland A, Giovino GA, Hastrup JL, Bauer JE, Bansal MA. Are smokers adequately informed about the health risks of smoking and medicinal nicotine? Nicotine Tob Res. 2004;6: 333–340.
    1. Smith P, Bansal-Travers M, O'Connor R, Brown A, Banthin C, Guardino-Colket S, Cummings KM. Correcting Over 50 Years of Tobacco Industry Misinformation. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2011;40: 690–698. 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.01.020
    1. Hornik R, Yanovitzky I. Using Theory to Design Evaluations of Communication Campaigns: The Case of the National Youth Anti-Drug Media Campaign. Commun Theory. 2003;13: 204–224. 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2003.tb00289.x
    1. Cohen E, Shumate M, Gold A. Anti-Smoking Media Campaign Messages: Theory and Practice. Health Commun. 2007;22: 91–102. 10.1080/10410230701453884
    1. Green E, Courage C, Rushton L. Reducing domestic exposure to environmental tobacco smoke: a review of attitudes and behaviours. J Royal Soc Prom Health. 2003;123: 46–51
    1. Strasser A, Cappella J, Jepson C, Fishbein M, Tang K, Han E, Lerman C. Experimental evaluation of antitobacco PSAs: Effects of message content and format on physiological and behavioral outcomes. Nicotine Tob Res. 2009;11: 293–302. 10.1093/ntr/ntn026
    1. Farrelly M, Niederdeppe J, Yarsevich J. Youth tobacco prevention mass media campaigns: past, present, and future directions. Tob Cont. 2003;12: 35–47.
    1. Hornik R. Public Health Communication. New York: Routledge; 2002.
    1. Warner K. The effects of the anti-smoking campaign on cigarette consumption. Am J Public Health. 1977;67: 645–650. 10.2105/ajph.67.7.645
    1. Bal D, Kizer K, Felten P, Mozar H, Niemeyer D. Reducing Tobacco Consumption in California: Development of a Statewide Anti-Tobacco Use Campaign. JAMA. 1990;264: 1570–1574.
    1. Hsieh C, Yen L, Liu J, Chyongchiou J. Smoking, health knowledge, and anti-smoking campaigns: An empirical study in Taiwan. J Health Econ. 1996;15: 87–104. 10.1016/0167-6296(95)00033-x
    1. Popham W, Potter L, Bal D, Johnson M, Duerr J, Quinn V. Do anti-smoking media campaigns help smokers quit? Public Health Rep. 1993;108: 510–513.
    1. McVey D, Stapleton J. Can anti-smoking television advertising affect smoking behaviour? Controlled trial of the Health Education Authority for England’s anti-smoking TV campaign. Tobacco Control. 2000;9: 273–282. 10.1136/tc.9.3.273
    1. Niederdeppe J, Fiore MC, Baker TB, Smith SS. Smoking-Cessation Media Campaigns and Their Effectiveness Among Socioeconomically Advantaged and Disadvantaged Populations. Am J Public Health. 2008;98: 916–924. 10.2105/AJPH.2007.117499
    1. Gibson L, Parvanta S, Jeong M, Hornik RC. Evaluation of a mass media campaign promoting using help to quit smoking. Am J Prev Med. 2014;46: 487–495. 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.01.011
    1. Lee S, Cappella J, Lerman C, Strasser A. Effects of Smoking Cues and Argument Strength of Antismoking Advertisements on Former Smokers’ Self-efficacy, Attitude, and Intention to Refrain from Smoking. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15: 527–533. 10.1093/ntr/nts171
    1. Reinhard M, Schindler S, Raabe V, Stahlberg D, Messner M. Less is sometimes more: How repetition of an antismoking advertisement affects attitudes toward smoking and source credibility. Soc Infl. 2014;9: 116–132.
    1. Chang C. Psychological Motives Versus Health Concerns: Predicting Smoking Attitudes and Promoting Antismoking Attitudes. Health Communication. 2009;24: 1–11. 10.1080/10410230802465241
    1. Henriksen L, Dauphine A, Wang Y, Fortmann S. Industry sponsored anti-smoking ads and adolescent reactance: test of a boomerang effect. Tob Cont. 2006;15: 13–18.
    1. Bradley B, Field M, Healy H, Mogg K. Do the affective properties of smoking-related cues influence attentional and approach biases in cigarette smokers? J Psychopharmacol. 2006;22: 737–745.
    1. Sayette A, Hufford M. Effects of Smoking Urge on Generation of Smoking‐Related Information. J App Soc Psych. 2006;27: 1395–1405.
    1. Segerstrom S, Mcarthy W, Caskey N, Gross T, Murray J. Optimistic Bias Among Cigarette Smokers. J App Soc Psych. 2006;23: 1606–1618.
    1. Arnett J. Optimistic bias in adolescent and adult smokers and nonsmokers. Add Behav. 2000;25: 625–632.
    1. Weinstein N, Marcus S, Moser R. Smokers’ unrealistic optimism about their risk. Tob Cont. 2005;14: 55–59.
    1. Weinstein N. Accuracy of smokers’ risk perceptions. Ann Behav Med. 1998;20: 135–140. 10.1007/BF02884459
    1. Dillard A, McCaul K, Klein W. Unrealistic Optimism in Smokers: Implications for Smoking Myth Endorsement and Self-Protective Motivation. J Health Commun. 2006;11: 93–102.
    1. Katz E, Lazarsfeld P. Personal Influence, the Part Played by People in the Flow of Mass Communications. New York: Transaction Publishers; 1966.
    1. Watts D, Dodds P. Influentials, Networks, and Public Opinion Formation. Journal of Consumer Research. 2007;34: 441–458.
    1. Prochaska J, Pechmann C, Kim R, Leonhardt M. Twitter = quitter? An analysis of Twitter quit smoking social networks. Tob Cont. 2012;21: 447–449.
    1. Robalino J, Macy M. Peer Effects on Adolescent Smoking: Are Popular Teens More Influential? PLoS ONE. 2018; 13(7): e0189360 10.1371/journal.pone.0189360
    1. Gough A, Hunter R, Ajao O, Jurek A, McKeown G, Hong J, et al. Tweet for Behavior Change: Using Social Media for the Dissemination of Public Health Messages. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2017;3: e14 10.2196/publichealth.6313
    1. Jeong M, Tan A, Brennan E, Gibson L, Hornik RC. Talking About Quitting: Interpersonal Communication as a Mediator of Campaign Effects on Smokers’ Quit Behaviors. J Health Commun. 2015;20: 1196–1205. 10.1080/10810730.2015.1018620
    1. Jamieson K, Cappella J. Echo Chamber: Rush Limbaugh and the Conservative Media Establishment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008.
    1. Guilbeault D, Becker J, Centola D. Social learning and partisan bias in the interpretation of climate trends. PNAS. 2018;115: 9714–9719. 10.1073/pnas.1722664115
    1. Christakis N, Fowler J. The Collective Dynamics of Smoking in a Large Social Network. New England Journal of Medicine. 2008;358: 2249–2258. 10.1056/NEJMsa0706154
    1. Poland B, Frohlich K, Haines R, Mykhalovskiy R, Sparks R. The social context of smoking: the next frontier in tobacco control? Tob Cont. 2006;15: 59–63.
    1. Mercken L, Snijders T, Steglich C, Vartianen E, Vries H. Dynamics of adolescent friendship networks and smoking behavior. Soc Net. 2010;32: 72–82.
    1. Mercken L, Snijders T, Steglich C, Vries H. Dynamics of adolescent friendship networks and smoking behavior: Social network analyses in six European countries. Soc Sci Med. 2009;69: 1506–1514. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.08.003
    1. Cobb N, Graham A, Byron J, Niaura R, Abrams D. Online Social Networks and Smoking Cessation: A Scientific Research Agenda. J Med Internet Res. 2011;13: e119 10.2196/jmir.1911
    1. Mutz D. Cross-cutting Social Networks: Testing Democratic Theory in Practice. American Political Science Review. 2002;96: 111–126.
    1. Gibson B. Nonsmokers’ Attributions for the Outcomes of Smokers: Some Potential Consequences of the Stigmatization of Smokers1. J App Soc Psych. 1998;28: 581–594.
    1. Evans-Polce R, Castaldelli-Maia J, Schomerus G, Evans-Lacko S. The downside of tobacco control? Smoking and self-stigma: A systematic review. Soc Sci Med. 2015;145: 26–34. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.09.026
    1. Stuber J, Meyer I, Link B. Stigma, prejudice, discrimination and health. Soc Sci Med. 2008;67: 351–357. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.023
    1. Graham H. Smoking, Stigma and Social Class. J Soc Pol. 2012;41: 83–99.
    1. Etter J. Internet-based smoking cessation programs. Int J Med Info. 2006;75: 110–116.
    1. Cobb N, Graham A, Abrams D. Social Network Structure of a Large Online Community for Smoking Cessation. Am J Public Health. 2010;100: 1282–1289. 10.2105/AJPH.2009.165449
    1. Zhang J, Brackbill D, Yang S, Centola D. Efficacy and causal mechanism of an online social media intervention to increase physical activity: Results of a randomized controlled trial. Prev Med Rep. 2015;2: 651–657. 10.1016/j.pmedr.2015.08.005
    1. Zhang J, Brackbill D, Yang S, Becker J, Herbert N, Centola D. Support or competition? How online social networks increase physical activity: A randomized controlled trial. Prev Med Rep. 2016;4: 453–458. 10.1016/j.pmedr.2016.08.008
    1. Centola D. The Spread of Behavior in an Online Social Network Experiment. Science. 2010;329: 1194–97. 10.1126/science.1185231
    1. Centola D. How Behavior Spreads. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 2018.
    1. Becker J, Brackbill D, Centola D. Network dynamics of social influence in the wisdom of crowds. PNAS. 2017;114: 5070–5076.
    1. Noether G. Sample Size Determination for Some Common Nonparametric Tests. J Am. Stat. Assoc. 1987; 87: 645–47.
    1. Orentlicher D. The FDA’s Graphic Tobacco Warnings and the First Amendment. New England Journal of Medicine. 2013;369: 204–206. 10.1056/NEJMp1304513
    1. Hsieh C. Health Risk and the Decision to Quit Smoking. Applied Economics (1998); 30: 795–804.
    1. Leventhal H., Glynn K., Fleming R. Is the Smoking Decision an ‘Informed Choice’?: Effect of Smoking Risk Factors on Smoking Beliefs. JAMA 1987; 257: 3373–76.
    1. Prelec D, Seung H, McCoy J. A solution to the single-question crowd wisdom problem. Nature. 2017; 541; 532–535. 10.1038/nature21054
    1. March J, Simon H. Organizations. New York: Wiley-Blackwell; 1993.
    1. Lebreton M, Abitbol R, Daunizeau J, Pessiglione M. Automatic integration of confidence in the brain valuation signal. Nat Neuro. 2015;18: 1159–1167.
    1. Corner A, Whitmarsh L, Xenias D. Uncertainty, scepticism and attitudes towards climate change: biased assimilation and attitude polarization. Clim Change. 2012;114:463–478.
    1. Yaniv I, Choshen-Hillel S, Milyavsky M. Spurious consensus and opinion revision: why might people be more confidence in their less accurate judgments? J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2009; 35; 558–563. 10.1037/a0014589
    1. Bénabou R, Tirole J. Self-Confidence and Personal Motivation. Q J Econ. 2002; 117: 871–915.
    1. Moore D, Healy P. The Trouble with Overconfidence. Psych Rev. 2008; 115: 502–517.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnere