Rehabilitation of executive functioning in patients with frontal lobe brain damage with goal management training

Brian Levine, Tom A Schweizer, Charlene O'Connor, Gary Turner, Susan Gillingham, Donald T Stuss, Tom Manly, Ian H Robertson, Brian Levine, Tom A Schweizer, Charlene O'Connor, Gary Turner, Susan Gillingham, Donald T Stuss, Tom Manly, Ian H Robertson

Abstract

Executive functioning deficits due to brain disease affecting frontal lobe functions cause significant real-life disability, yet solid evidence in support of executive functioning interventions is lacking. Goal Management Training (GMT), an executive functioning intervention that draws upon theories concerning goal processing and sustained attention, has received empirical support in studies of patients with traumatic brain injury, normal aging, and case studies. GMT promotes a mindful approach to complex real-life tasks that pose problems for patients with executive functioning deficits, with a main goal of periodically stopping ongoing behavior to monitor and adjust goals. In this controlled trial, an expanded version of GMT was compared to an alternative intervention, Brain Health Workshop that was matched to GMT on non-specific characteristics that can affect intervention outcome. Participants included 19 individuals in the chronic phase of recovery from brain disease (predominantly stroke) affecting frontal lobe function. Outcome data indicated specific effects of GMT on the Sustained Attention to Response Task as well as the Tower Test, a visuospatial problem-solving measure that reflected far transfer of training effects. There were no significant effects on self-report questionnaires, likely owing to the complexity of these measures in this heterogeneous patient sample. Overall, these data support the efficacy of GMT in the rehabilitation of executive functioning deficits.

Keywords: executive functioning; frontal lobe; rehabilitation; stroke; traumatic brain injury mindfulness.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Effects of GMT vs. BHW on SART errors of omission (top left), SART coefficient of variation (top right), Tower Test Achievement Score (bottom left) and Tower Test rule violations per item (bottom right). *Significantly different from baseline for GMT. The symbols represent the mean values and the error bars represent the SEM.

References

    1. Broadbent D. E., Cooper P. F., FitzGerald P., Parkes K. R. (1982). The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 21(Pt 1), 1–16
    1. Burgess P. W., Alderman N., Evans J., Emslie H., Wilson B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 4, 547–558
    1. Burgess P. W., Alderman N., Evans J. J., Wilson B. A., Emslie H. (1996). “The dysexecutive questionnaire,” in Behavioral Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome, eds Wilson B. A., Alderman N., Burgess P. W., Emslie H., Evans J. J. (Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company; ).
    1. Cicerone K. D., Dahlberg C., Kalmar K., Langenbahn D. M., Malec J. F., Bergquist T. F., Felicetti T., Giacino J. T., Harley J. P., Harrington D. E., Herzog J., Kneipp S., Laatsch L., Morse P. A. (2000). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommendations for clinical practice. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 81, 1596–1615
    1. Cicerone K. D., Dahlberg C., Malec J. F., Langenbahn D. M., Felicetti T., Kneipp S., Ellmo W., Kalmar K., Giacino J. T., Harley J. P., Laatsch L., Morse P. A., Catanese J. (2005). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 86, 1681–1692 [See comment].
    1. Cohen J. D., Servan-Schreiber D. (1992). Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychol. Bull. 99, 45–77
    1. Corbetta M., Shulman G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215
    1. Delis D. C., Kaplan E., Kramer J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning System (D-KEFS). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation
    1. Duncan J., Emslie H., Williams P., Johnson R., Freer C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe: the organization of goal-directed behavior. Cogn. Psychol. 30, 257–303
    1. Fish J., Evans J. J., Nimmo M., Martin E., Kersel D., Bateman A., Wilson B. A., Manly T. (2007). Rehabilitation of executive dysfunction following brain injury: “content-free” cueing improves everyday prospective memory performance. Neuropsychologia 45, 1318–133010.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.015
    1. Jaeggi S. M., Buschkuehl M., Jonides J., Perrig W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 6829–6833
    1. Kabat-Zinn J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living. New York: Dell Publishing
    1. Levine B., Dawson D., Boutet I., Schwartz M. L., Stuss D. T. (2000a). Assessment of strategic self-regulation in traumatic brain injury: its relationship to injury severity and psychosocial outcome. Neuropsychology 14, 491–50010.1037//0894-4105.14.4.491
    1. Levine B., Robertson I. H., Clare L., Carter G., Hong J., Wilson B. A., Duncan J., Stuss D. T. (2000b). Rehabilitation of executive functioning: an experimental-clinical validation of goal management training. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 6, 299–31210.1017/S1355617700633052
    1. Levine B., Downey-Lamb M. (2002). “Design and evaluation of intervention experiments,” in Neuropsychological Interventions: Emerging Treatment and Management Models for Neuropsychological Impairments, ed. Eslinger P. J. (New York: Guilford Publications; ), 80–104
    1. Levine B., Stuss D. T., Milberg W. P., Alexander M. P., Schwartz M., Macdonald R. (1998). The effects of focal and diffuse brain damage on strategy application: evidence from focal lesions, traumatic brain injury, and normal aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 4, 247–264
    1. Levine B., Stuss D. T., Winocur G., Binns M. A., Fahy L., Mandic M., Bridges K., Robertson I. H. (2007). Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: effects on strategic behavior in relation to goal management. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 13, 143–152
    1. Levine B., Turner G. R., Stuss D. T. (2008). “Rehabilitation of frontal lobe functions,” in Cognitive Neurorehabilitation, Evidence and Applications, eds Stuss D. T., Winocur G., Robertson I. H. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; ), 464–486
    1. Manly T., Hawkins K., Evans J., Woldt K., Robertson I. H. (2002). Rehabilitation of executive function: facilitation of effective goal management on complex tasks using periodic auditory alerts. Neuropsychologia 40, 271–28110.1016/S0028-3932(01)00094-X
    1. Mayberg H. S. (1997). Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 9, 471–481
    1. McDonald W. I., Ron M. A. (1999). Multiple sclerosis: the disease and its manifestations. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 354, 1615–1622
    1. McIntosh A. R., Kovacevic N., Itier R. J. (2008). Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4, e1000106.10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106
    1. Neary D., Snowden J. S., Gustafson L., Passant U., Stuss D. T., Black S., Freedman M., Kertesz A., Robert P. H., Albert M., Boone K., Miller B. L., Cummings J., Benson D. F. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51, 1546–1554
    1. Norman D. A., Shallice T. (1986). “Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behaviour,” in Consciousness and Self-regulation, eds Schwartz G. E., Shapiro D. (New York: Plenum Press; ), 1–18
    1. O'Connell R. G., Dockree P. M., Bellgrove M. A., Turin A., Ward S., Foxe J. J., Robertson I. H. (2009). Two types of action error: electrophysiological evidence for separable inhibitory and sustained attention neural mechanisms producing error on go/no-go tasks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 93–104
    1. O'Connor C., Robertson I. H., Levine B. (in press). The prosthetics of vigilant attention: random cuing cuts processing demands. Neuropsychology.
    1. Persson J., Reuter-Lorenz P. A. (2008). Gaining control: training executive function and far transfer of the ability to resolve interference. Psychol. Sci. 19, 881–888
    1. Posner M. I., Petersen S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25–42
    1. Raz N. (2000). “Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: integration of structural and functional findings,” in The handbook of Aging and Cognition, 2nd Edn, eds Craik F. I. M., Salthouse T. A. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; ), 1–90
    1. Robertson I. H. (1996). Goal Management Training: A Clinical Manual. Cambridge: PsyConsult
    1. Robertson I. H., Garavan H. (2000). “Vigilant attention,” in The New Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga M. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; ), 563–578
    1. Robertson I. H., Manly T., Andrade J., Baddeley B. T., Yiend J. (1997). “Oops!”: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 35, 747–75810.1016/S0028-3932(97)00015-8
    1. Rohling M. L., Faust M. E., Beverly B., Demakis G. (2009). Effectiveness of cognitive rehabilitation following acquired brain injury: a meta-analytic re-examination of Cicerone et al.’s (2000, 2005) systematic reviews. Neuropsychology 23, 20–3910.1037/a0013659
    1. Schweizer T. A., Levine B., Rewilak D., O'Connor C., Turner G., Alexander M. P., Cusimano M., Manly T., Robertson I. H., Stuss D. T. (2008). Rehabilitation of executive functioning after focal damage to the cerebellum. Neurorehabil. Neural. Repair 22, 72–77
    1. Segal Z. V., Williams J. M. G., Teasdale J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based Cognitive Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse xiv. New York, NY: Guilford Press, 351
    1. Shallice T. (1982). Specific impairments in planning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B 298, 199–209
    1. Shallice T., Burgess P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain 114, 727–74110.1093/brain/114.2.727
    1. Stuss D. T., Alexander M. P. (2007). Is there a dysexecutive syndrome? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, Biol. Sci. 362, 901–915
    1. Stuss D. T., Levine B. (2002). Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 401–43310.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135220
    1. Stuss D. T., Murphy K. J., Binns M. A., Alexander M. P. (2003). Staying on the job: the frontal lobes control individual performance variability. Brain 126(Pt 11), 2363–2380
    1. Stuss D. T., Pogue J., Buckle L., Bondar J. (1994). Characterization of stability of performance in patients with traumatic brain injury: variability and consistency on reaction time tests. Neuropsychology 8, 316–32410.1037/0894-4105.8.3.316
    1. Stuss D. T., Stethem L. L., Hugenholtz H., Picton T., Pivik J., Richard M. T. (1989). Reaction time after traumatic brain injury: fatigue, divided and focused attention, and consistency of performance. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 52, 742–748
    1. Swartz R. H., Stuss D. T., Gao F., Black S. E. (2008). Independent cognitive effects of atrophy and diffuse subcortical and thalamico-cortical cerebrovascular disease in dementia. Stroke 39, 822–83010.1161/STROKEAHA.107.491936
    1. Tranel D., Anderson S. W., Benton A. (1994). “Development of the concept of “executive function” and its relationship to the frontal lobes,” in Handbook of Neuropsychology, eds Boller F., Grafman J. (Amsterdam: Elsevier; ), 125–148
    1. van Hooren S. A., Valentijn S. A., Bosma H., Ponds R. W., van Boxtel M. P., Levine B., Robertson I. H., Jolles J. (2007). Effect of a structured course involving goal management training in older adults: a randomised controlled trial. Patient Educ. Couns. 65, 205–213

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnere