A randomized controlled trial of cognitive training using a visual speed of processing intervention in middle aged and older adults

Fredric D Wolinsky, Mark W Vander Weg, M Bryant Howren, Michael P Jones, Megan M Dotson, Fredric D Wolinsky, Mark W Vander Weg, M Bryant Howren, Michael P Jones, Megan M Dotson

Abstract

Background: Age-related cognitive decline is common and may lead to substantial difficulties and disabilities in everyday life. We hypothesized that 10 hours of visual speed of processing training would prevent age-related declines and potentially improve cognitive processing speed.

Methods: Within two age bands (50-64 and ≥ 65) 681 patients were randomized to (a) three computerized visual speed of processing training arms (10 hours on-site, 14 hours on-site, or 10 hours at-home) or (b) an on-site attention control group using computerized crossword puzzles for 10 hours. The primary outcome was the Useful Field of View (UFOV) test, and the secondary outcomes were the Trail Making (Trails) A and B Tests, Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Stroop Color and Word Tests, Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT), and the Digit Vigilance Test (DVT), which were assessed at baseline and at one year. 620 participants (91%) completed the study and were included in the analyses. Linear mixed models were used with Blom rank transformations within age bands.

Results: All intervention groups had (p<0.05) small to medium standardized effect size improvements on UFOV (Cohen's d = -0.322 to -0.579, depending on intervention arm), Trails A (d = -0.204 to -0.265), Trails B (d = -0.225 to -0.320), SDMT (d = 0.263 to 0.351), and Stroop Word (d = 0.240 to 0.271). Converted to years of protection against age-related cognitive declines, these effects reflect 3.0 to 4.1 years on UFOV, 2.2 to 3.5 years on Trails A, 1.5 to 2.0 years on Trails B, 5.4 to 6.6 years on SDMT, and 2.3 to 2.7 years on Stroop Word.

Conclusion: Visual speed of processing training delivered on-site or at-home to middle-aged or older adults using standard home computers resulted in stabilization or improvement in several cognitive function tests. Widespread implementation of this intervention is feasible.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT-01165463.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT01165463.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. CONSORT Flow Chart for the…
Figure 1. CONSORT Flow Chart for the IHAMS Study.
Figure 2. How the Visual Speed of…
Figure 2. How the Visual Speed of Processing Training Program Appears to the User.
Figure 3. Plots of Blom Rank Transformed…
Figure 3. Plots of Blom Rank Transformed Means on the Useful Field of View by Treatment Group at Baseline and One Year.

References

    1. Birren JE, Woods AM, Williams MV (1980) Behavioral slowing with age: causes, organization, and consequences of slowing. In Poon LW, editor, Aging in the 1980s: Psychological issues. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 293–308.
    1. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Myers RS, Roenker DL, Cissell GM, et al. (2002) Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions. Gerontol 48: 329–340.
    1. Madden DJ (1992) Four to ten milliseconds per year: Age-related slowing of visual word identification. J Gerontol: Psych Sci Soc Sci 47: P59–P68.
    1. Roenker DL, Cissell GM, Ball KK, Wadley VG, Edwards JD (2003) Speed-of-processing and driving simulator training result in improved driving performance. Hum Factors 45: 218–233.
    1. Burdick DJ, Rosenblatt A, Samus QM, Steele C, Baker A, et al. (2005) Predictors of functional impairment in residents of assisted living facilities: The Maryland Assisted Living Study. J Gerontol: Biol Sci Med Sci 60A: 258–264.
    1. Cahn-Weiner DA, Malloy PF, Boyle PA, Marran M, Salloway S (2000) Prediction of functional status from neuropsychological tests in community-dwelling elderly individuals. Clin Neuropsychol 14: 187–195.
    1. Hertzog C, Kramer AF, Wilson RS, Lindenberger U (2008) Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychol Sci Pub Interest 9: 1–65.
    1. Salthouse TA (2012) Consequences of age-related cognitive declines. Ann Rev Psychol 63: 201–226.
    1. Wang H, Preston SH (2009) Forecasting United States mortality using cohort smoking histories. PNAS 106: 393–398.
    1. National Academy of Sciences (2006) Workshop on identifying new interventions to extend disability decline in elderly populations. Bethesda, MD: National Academies of Science. Available: . Accessed 2013 Apr 9.
    1. National Institutes of Health (2011) PA 11–123: Translational research to help older adults maintain their health and independence in the community. Washington, DC: NIH.
    1. Institute of Medicine (2012) Living well with chronic illness: A call for public health action. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
    1. Salthouse TA (2010) Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 16: 754–760.
    1. Salthouse TA (2009) When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging 30: 507–514.
    1. Salthouse TA (2010) Influence of age on practice effects in longitudinal neurocognitive change. Neuropsychol 24: 563–572.
    1. Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Clymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, et al. (2012) Timing of onset of cognitive decline: Results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ 343: 1–8.
    1. Draganski B, Gaser C, Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Winkler J, et al. (2006) Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J Neurosci 26: 6314–6317.
    1. Doidge N (2007) The brain that changes itself: Stories of personal triumph from the frontiers of brain science. New York: Viking Penguin.
    1. Edwards JD, Vance DE, Wadley VG, Cissell GM, Roenker DL, et al. (2005) Reliability and validity of Useful Field of View test scores as administered by personal computer. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 27: 529–543.
    1. Wadley VG, Benz RL, Ball KK, Roenker DL, Edwards JD, et al. (2006) Development and evaluation of home-based speed of processing training for older adults. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 87: 757–763.
    1. Ball KK, Edwards JD, Ross LA (2007) The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 62B: 19–31.
    1. Jobe JB, Smith DM, Ball KK, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, et al. (2001) ACTIVE: A cognitive intervention trial to promote independence in older adults. Controlled Clin Trials 22: 453–479.
    1. Ball KK, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, et al. (2002) Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288: 2271–2281.
    1. Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, Ball KK, Elias J, et al. (2006) Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA 296: 2805–2814.
    1. Jones RN, Marsiske M, Ball K, Rebok G, Willis SL, et al... (2012) The ACTIVE cognitive training interventions and trajectories of performance among older adults. J Health Aging. doi: 10.1177/0898264312461938.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Mahncke HW, Kosinksi M, Unverzagt FW, Smith DM, et al. (2009) The ACTIVE cognitive training trial and predicted medical expenditures. BMC Health Serv Res 9(109): 1–9.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Unverzagt FW, Smith DM, Jones R, Wright E, et al. (2006) The effects of the ACTIVE cognitive training interventions on clinically relevant declines in health-related quality of life. J Gerontol: Psychol Sci Soc Sci 61B: S281–S287.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Unverzagt FW, Smith DM, Jones R, Stoddard A, et al. (2006) The ACTIVE cognitive training trial and health-related quality of life: Protection that lasts for five years. J Gerontol: Bio Sci Med Sci 61A: 1324–1329.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Martin R, Unverzagt FW, Ball KK, et al. (2009) The effect of speed of processing training on depressive symptoms in ACTIVE. J Gerontol: Bio Sci Med Sci 64A: 468–472.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Mahncke HW, Vander Weg MW, Martin R, Unverzagt FW, et al. (2009) The ACTIVE cognitive training interventions and the onset of and recovery from suspected clinical depression. J Gerontol: Psychol Sci Soc Sci 64B: 577–585.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Mahncke HW, Vander Weg MW, Martin R, Unverzagt FW, et al. (2010) Speed of processing training improves self-rated health in older adults: Enduring effects observed in the multi-site ACTIVE study. Int Psychogeriatr 22: 470–478.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Martin R, Unverzagt FW, Willis SL, et al. (2010) Cognitive training improves internal locus of control among older adults. J Gerontol: Psych Sci Soc Sci 65B: 470–478.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Howren MB, Jones MP, Martin R, et al. (2011) Protocol for a randomized controlled trial to improve cognitive functioning in older adults: The Iowa Healthy and Active Minds Study. BMJ Open 1(218): 1–10.
    1. Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Howren MB, Jones MP, Martin R, et al. (2011) Interim results from a randomized controlled trial to improve visual processing speed in older adults: The Iowa Healthy and Active Minds Study. BMJ Open 1(225): 1–13.
    1. Pfeiffer EA (1975) A short portable mental status questionnaire for the assessment of organic brain deficit in elderly patients. J Am Geriatr Soc 23: 433–441.
    1. Reitan RM, Wolfson D (1985) The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Therapy and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychological Press.
    1. Smith A (1982) Symbol digit modality test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
    1. Golden CJ (1978) The Stroop color and word test. Chicago, IL: Stoelting Company.
    1. Benton AL, Hamsher K, Silvan AB (1994) Multilingual aphasia examination (3rd edition) Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.
    1. Lewis R, Rennick PM (1979) Manual for the repeatable cognitive-perceptual-motor battery. Gross Point, MI: Axon.
    1. Blom G (1958) Statistical estimates and transformed beta-variables. New York, NY: Wiley.
    1. Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112: 155–159.
    1. Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S (2000) Applied logistic regression. New York: Wiley.
    1. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Vance DE, Wood K, Roenker DL, et al. (2005) The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance. Aging Ment Health 9: 262–271.
    1. Clay OJ, Wadley VG, Edwards JD, Roth DL, Roenker DL, et al. (2005) Cumulative meta-analysis of the relationship between useful field of view and driving performance in older adults: Current and future implications. Optom Vis Sci 82: 724–731.
    1. Ball K, Edwards JD, Ross LA, McGwin G (2011) Cognitive training decreases motor vehicle collision involvement of older drivers. J Am Geriatr Soc 58: 2107–2113.
    1. Salthouse TA (1996) The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev 103: 403–428.
    1. Zimprich DM (2002) Can longitudinal changes in processing speed explain longitudinal changes in fluid intelligence? Psychol Aging 17: 690–695.
    1. Wahl HW, Schmitt M, Danner D, Coppin A (2010) Is the emergence of functional ability decline in early old age related to change in speed of cognitive processing and also to change in personality? J Aging Health 22: 691–712.
    1. Salthouse TA (1994) The nature of the influence of speed on adult age-differences in cognition. Develop Psychol 30: 240–259.
    1. Edwards JD, Ross LA, Wadley VG, Clay OJ, Crowe M, et al. (2006) The useful field of view test: Normative data for older adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 21: 275–286.
    1. Tombaugh TN (2004) Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 19: 203–214.
    1. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW (2004) Neuropsychological assessment, 4th ed. New York: Oxford.
    1. Vogel A, Stokholm J, Jorgensen K (2012) Performances on Symbol Digit Modalities Test, and modified Stroop test in a healthy Danish sample. Neuropsychol Cognition 28. In press.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnere