Divergent effects of brief contemplative practices in response to an acute stressor: A randomized controlled trial of brief breath awareness, loving-kindness, gratitude or an attention control practice

Matthew J Hirshberg, Simon B Goldberg, Stacey M Schaefer, Lisa Flook, David Findley, Richard J Davidson, Matthew J Hirshberg, Simon B Goldberg, Stacey M Schaefer, Lisa Flook, David Findley, Richard J Davidson

Abstract

Mindfulness practices are increasingly being utilized as a method for cultivating well-being. The term mindfulness is often used as an umbrella for a variety of different practices and many mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) contain multiple styles of practice. Despite the diversity of practices within MBIs, few studies have investigated whether constituent practices produce specific effects. We randomized 156 undergraduates to one of four brief practices: breath awareness, loving-kindness, gratitude, or to an attention control condition. We assessed practice effects on affect following brief training, and effects on affect and behavior after provocation with a stressor (i.e., Cold pressor test). Results indicate that gratitude training significantly improved positive affect compared to breath awareness (d = 0.58) and loving-kindness led to significantly greater reductions in implicit negative affect compared to the control condition (d = 0.59) immediately after brief practice. In spite of gains in positive affect, the gratitude group demonstrated increased reactivity to the stressor, reporting the CPT as significantly more aversive than the control condition (d = 0.46) and showing significantly greater increases in negative affect compared to the breath awareness, loving-kindness, and control groups (ds = 0.55, 0.60, 0.65, respectively). Greater gains in implicit positive affect following gratitude training predicted decreased post-stressor likability ratings of novel neutral faces compared to breath awareness, loving-kindness, and control groups (ds = - 0.39, -0.40, -0.33, respectively) as well. Moreover, the gratitude group was significantly less likely to donate time than the loving-kindness group in an ecologically valid opportunity to provide unrewarded support. These data suggest that different styles of contemplative practice may produce different effects in the context of brief, introductory practice and these differences may be heightened by stress. Implications for the study of contemplative practices are discussed.

Conflict of interest statement

Richard J. Davidson is the founder, president, and serves on the board of directors for the non-profit organization, Healthy Minds Innovations, Inc. In addition, Dr. Davidson served on the board of directors for the Mind & Life Institute from 1992-2017. No donors, either anonymous or identified, have participated in the design, conduct, or reporting of research results in this manuscript. We confirm that our Competing Interest statement does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.

Figures

Fig 1. Study design.
Fig 1. Study design.
Fig 2. CONSORT diagram.
Fig 2. CONSORT diagram.
Fig 3. Training changes in positive affect.
Fig 3. Training changes in positive affect.
Notes: *** = p < .001, ** = p < .01, * = p < .05, t = p < .10. Loving-kindness = LK; Breath awareness = BA; Gratitude = GT; Control = CT.
Fig 4. Training changes in negative affect.
Fig 4. Training changes in negative affect.
Notes: *** = p < .001, ** = p < .01, * = p < .05, t = p < .10. Loving-kindness = LK; Breath awareness = BA; Gratitude = GT; Control = CT.
Fig 5. Cold pressor aversiveness ratings.
Fig 5. Cold pressor aversiveness ratings.
Notes: *** = p < .001, ** = p < .01, * = p < .05, t = p < .10; 4A. Loving-kindness = LK; Breath awareness = BA; Gratitude = GT; Control = CT.
Fig 6. Cold pressor effects on negative…
Fig 6. Cold pressor effects on negative affect.
Notes: *** = p < .001, ** = p < .01, * = p < .05, t = p < .10; 4A. Loving-kindness = LK; Breath awareness = BA; Gratitude = GT; Control = CT.
Fig 7. Practice induced positive affect x…
Fig 7. Practice induced positive affect x GROUP interaction predicts stressor-related negative affect.
Notes: Explicit positive affect following practice in GT predicts stressor-induced increases in negative affect whereas in BA practice-related explicit positive affect gains are protective against stressor-induced negative affect t(68) = 3.50, p < .002, d = 0.81 CI[.33, 1.29] (corrected).
Fig 8. Positive affect training gains x…
Fig 8. Positive affect training gains x GROUP predict post-CPT face likability ratings.
Notes: Implicit positive affect following practice in GT predicts stressor-induced decreases in face likability ratings whereas in LK, BA, and CT practice-related implicit positive affect gains predict higher likability ratings GT-LK t(76) = -2.68, p = .020, d = -0.40 CI[0.1, 0.70], GT-BA t(68) = -2.51, p = .020, d = -0.39 CI[-0.71, -0.07], and GT-CT t(73) = -2.12, p = .036, d = -0.33 [-0.65, -0.01] (corrected).
Fig 9. Training effects on generosity.
Fig 9. Training effects on generosity.
*** = p < .001, ** = p < .01, * = p < .05, t = p < .10; 4A. Loving-kindness = LK; Breath awareness = BA; Gratitude = GT; Control = CT.

References

    1. Kabat-Zinn J. (2009). Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life Hachette Books.
    1. Creswell JD. Mindfulness interventions. Annual review of psychology. 2017;68:491–516.
    1. Goldberg SB, Tucker RP, Greene PA, Davidson RJ, Wampold BE, Kearney DJ, et al. Mindfulness-based interventions for psychiatric disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical psychology review. 2018; 59:52–60.
    1. Kabat-Zinn J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4(1), 33–47.
    1. Stahl B, Goldstein E. A mindfulness-based stress reduction workbook New Harbinger Publications; 2010.
    1. Wallace BA. The spectrum of Buddhist practice in the West. Westward Dharma: Buddhism Beyond Asia; 2002;34–50.
    1. Dahl CJ, Lutz A, Davidson RJ. Reconstructing and deconstructing the self: cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice. Trends Cogn Sci (Regul Ed). 2015. July 28; 515–523.
    1. Hutcherson CA, Seppala EM, Gross JJ. Loving-kindness meditation increases social connectedness. Emotion. 2008. October;8(5):720–4.
    1. Zeidan F, Johnson SK, Gordon NS, Goolkasian P. Effects of Brief and Sham Mindfulness Meditation on Mood and Cardiovascular Variables. Journal of Alternative & Complementary Medicine. 2010. August;16(8):867–73.
    1. Lumma A-L, Kok BE, Singer T. Is meditation always relaxing? Investigating heart rate, heart rate variability, experienced effort and likeability during training of three types of meditation. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2015;97(1):38–45.
    1. Raio CM, Hartley CA, Orederu TA, Li J, Phelps EA. Stress attenuates the flexible updating of aversive value. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2017;
    1. Forgas JP. Can negative affect eliminate the power of first impressions? Affective influences on primacy and recency effects in impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2011. March 1;47(2):425–9.
    1. Brown CC, Raio CM, Neta M. Cortisol responses enhance negative valence perception for ambiguous facial expressions. Scientific Reports. 2017;7(1):15107
    1. Steinbeis N, Engert V, Linz R, Singer T. The effects of stress and affiliation on social decision-making: Investigating the tend-and-befriend pattern. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015. December 1;62(Supplement C):138–48.
    1. Chiesa A, Serretti A, Jakobsen JC. Mindfulness: Top–down or bottom–up emotion regulation strategy? Clinical psychology review. 2013;33(1):82–96.
    1. Vago DR, Silbersweig DA. Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in human neuroscience [Internet]. 2012. [cited 2015 Oct 27];6 Available from:
    1. Engert V, Kok BE, Papassotiriou I, Chrousos GP, Singer T. Specific reduction in cortisol stress reactivity after social but not attention-based mental training. Science Advances. 2017. October 1;3(10):e1700495
    1. Zeidan F, Johnson SK, Gordon NS, Goolkasian P. Effects of Brief and Sham Mindfulness Meditation on Mood and Cardiovascular Variables. Journal of Alternative & Complementary Medicine. 2010. August;16(8):867–73.
    1. Law RW. An analogue study of loving-kindness meditation as a buffer against social stress The University of Arizona; 2011.
    1. Wood AM, Froh JJ, Geraghty AWA. Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical integration. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010. November 1;30(7):890–905.
    1. Watkins PC, Woodward K, Stone T, Kolts RL. Gratitude and happiness: Development of a measure of gratitude, and relationships with subjective well-being. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal. 2003;31(5):431–451.
    1. Schwabe L, Haddad L, Schachinger H. HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold-pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008. July 1;33(6):890–5.
    1. Fredrickson BL, Cohn MA, Coffey KA, Pek J, Finkel SM. Open hearts build lives: positive emotions, induced through loving-kindness meditation, build consequential personal resources. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2008;95(5):1045
    1. Brown CC, Raio CM, Neta M. Cortisol responses enhance negative valence perception for ambiguous facial expressions. Scientific Reports. 2017;7(1):15107
    1. Salzberg S. Guided meditations for love and wisdom: 14 essential practices. [CD]. Louisville, CO: Sounds True; 2009
    1. Hofmann SG, Grossman P, Hinton DE. Loving-kindness and compassion meditation: Potential for psychological interventions. Clinical psychology review. 2011;31(7):1126–1132.
    1. Reiss S, Peterson RA, Gursky DM, McNally RJ. Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behaviour research and therapy. 1986;24(1):1–8.
    1. McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang J-A. The grateful disposition: a conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2002;82(1):112
    1. Quirin M, Kazén M, Kuhl J. When nonsense sounds happy or helpless: the implicit positive and negative affect test (IPANAT). Journal of personality and social psychology. 2009;97(3):500
    1. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1988;54(6):1063
    1. Lovallo W. The cold pressor test and autonomic function: a review and integration. Psychophysiology. 1975;12(3):268–282.
    1. Lutz A, McFarlin DR, Perlman DM, Salomons TV, Davidson RJ. Altered anterior insula activation during anticipation and experience of painful stimuli in expert meditators. Neuroimage. 2013;64:538–546.
    1. Grupe DW, Schaefer SM, Lapate RC, Schoen AJ, Gresham LK, Mumford JA, et al. Behavioral and neural indices of affective coloring for neutral social stimuli. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018. March 1;13(3):310–20.
    1. Lapate RC, Rokers B, Li T, Davidson RJ. Nonconscious emotional activation colors first impressions: A regulatory role for conscious awareness. Psychological science. 2014;25(2):349–357.
    1. Messer K, Matas J, Kittler J, Luettin J, Maitre G. XM2VTSDB: The extended M2VTS database. In: Second international conference on audio and video-based biometric person authentication. 1999. p. 965–966.
    1. Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, et al. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry research. 2009;168(3):242–249.
    1. Beaupré, M. G., Cheung, N., & Hess, U. (2000).The Montreal Set of Facial Displays of Emotion [Slides]. (Available from Ursula Hess, Department of Psychology, University of Quebec at Montreal, P.O. Box 8888, Station “Centre-ville,” Montreal, Quebec H3C 3P8.)
    1. Qualtrics. (2015). Qualtrics: Provo, Utah.
    1. Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 3.0]. (2016). Retrieved from .
    1. Unsworth N, Heitz RP, Schrock JC, Engle RW. An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior research methods. 2005;37(3):498–505.
    1. Muraven M, Tice DM, Baumeister RF. Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1998;74(3):774 dx.doiorg/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252
    1. Goldberg SB, Flook L, Hirshberg MJ, Findley D, Kesebir P, Schaefer SM, et al. Getting a Grip on the Handgrip Task: Handgrip Duration Correlates with Neuroticism But Not Conscientiousness. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8:1367
    1. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the royal statistical society Series B (Methodological). 1995;289–300.
    1. Lakens D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in psychology. 2013;4
    1. RStudio Team. RStudio:Integrated development for R. RSTudio. 2015, Boston
    1. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang A-G. Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior research methods. 2009;41(4):1149–1160.
    1. Condon P, Desbordes G, Miller WB, DeSteno D. Meditation increases compassionate responses to suffering. Psychol Sci. 2013. October;24(10):2125–7.
    1. Przyrembel M, Singer T. Experiencing meditation–Evidence for differential effects of three contemplative mental practices in micro-phenomenological interviews. Consciousness and cognition. 2018;62:82–101.
    1. Wood AM, Maltby J, Gillett R, Linley PA, Joseph S. The role of gratitude in the development of social support, stress, and depression: Two longitudinal studies. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42(4):854–871.
    1. Emmons RA, McCullough ME. Counting blessings versus burdens: an experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2003;84(2):377

Source: PubMed

3
Abonnere