Gradual extinction reduces reinstatement

Youssef Shiban, Jasmin Wittmann, Mara Weißinger, Andreas Mühlberger, Youssef Shiban, Jasmin Wittmann, Mara Weißinger, Andreas Mühlberger

Abstract

The current study investigated whether gradually reducing the frequency of aversive stimuli during extinction can prevent the return of fear. Thirty-one participants of a three-stage procedure (acquisition, extinction and a reinstatement test on day 2) were randomly assigned to a standard extinction (SE) and gradual extinction (GE) procedure. The two groups differed only in the extinction procedure. While the SE group ran through a regular extinction process without any negative events, the frequency of the aversive stimuli during the extinction phase was gradually reduced for the GE group. The unconditioned stimulus (US) was an air blast (5 bar, 10 ms). A spider and a scorpion were used as conditioned stimuli (CS). The outcome variables were contingency ratings and physiological measures (skin conductance response, SCR and startle response). There were no differences found between the two groups for the acquisition and extinction phases concerning contingency ratings, SCR, or startle response. GE compared to SE significantly reduced the return of fear in the reinstatement test for the startle response but not for SCR or contingency ratings. This study was successful in translating the findings in rodent to humans. The results suggest that the GE process is suitable for increasing the efficacy of fear extinction.

Keywords: contingency ratings; gradual extinction; pavlovian fear conditioning; skin conductance response; startle response; virtual reality.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Virtual environment and virtual stimuli. (A) Virtual room where acquisition and extinction phases in a virtual reality (VR) took place. (B) Virtual room where reinstatement test in VR took place. (C) The presented virtual spider was used as an aversive conditioned stimulus (CS+). (D) The virtual scorpion was used as a non-aversive CS−.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Schematic procedure of the experiment. Each phase for the 2 days is given. Arrows represent the moments ratings were given. Each rating included a presentation of the CS+ and the CS−. Stimulus presentations for the ratings are not included in the numbers of CS+ and CS−.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Pattern of the presentation of the US during the extinction phase for the gradual group. Each box symbolizes the presentation of a CS+, which was paired with an US at the colored boxes.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Schematic procedure. The number of analyzed data for the measures in each phase of the experiment (acquisition phase, extinction phase, and reinstatement test) is given. Note: n, number of participants with analyzable data.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Contingency rating for CS+ and CS− for all phases for the Gradual and Standard groups. Note: CS+, stimulus with negative consequences; CS−, stimulus without negative consequences; Standard, the experimental group which participated in the Standard extinction (SE) process; Gradual, the experimental group which participated in the Gradual extinction (GE) process. Mean contingency ratings are given. Standard errors are presented as error bars.
Figure 6
Figure 6
Startle response for CS+ and CS− for all phases for the Gradual and Standard groups. Note: CS+, stimulus with negative consequences; CS−, stimulus without negative consequences; Standard, the experimental group which participated in the Standard Extinction process; Gradual, the experimental group which participated in the Gradual Extinction process. Mean startle responses are given. Standard errors are presented as error bars.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Comparison of SCR at all phases for the gradual and standard groups. Note: CS+, stimulus with negative consequences; CS-, stimulus without negative consequences; Standard, the experimental group which participated in the Standard Extinction process; Gradual, the experimental group which participated in the Gradual Extinction process. Mean startle responses are given. Standard errors are presented as error bars.

References

    1. Acheson D., Feifel D., de Wilde S., McKinney R., Lohr J., Risbrough V. (2013). The effect of intranasal oxytocin treatment on conditioned fear extinction and recall in a healthy human sample. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 229, 199–208. 10.1007/s00213-013-3099-4
    1. Anderson P. L., Zimand E., Hodges L. F., Rothbaum B. (2005). Cognitive behavioral therapy for public-speaking anxiety using virtual reality for exposure. Depress. Anxiety 22, 156–158. 10.1002/da.20090
    1. Atkinson R. L., Atkinson R. C., Smith E. E., Bem D. J., NolenHoeksema S. (1995). Introduction to psychology 10th Edm. Orlando: Harcourt College.
    1. Blumenthal T. D., Cuthbert B. N., Filion D. L., Hackley S., Lipp O. V., van Boxtel A. (2005). Committee report: guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic studies. Psychophysiology 42, 1–15. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00271.x
    1. Bouton M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn. Mem. 11, 485–494. 10.1101/lm.78804
    1. Brooks D. C., Bouton M. E. (1993). A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates spontaneous-recovery. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 19, 77–89. 10.1037/0097-7403.19.1.77
    1. Craske M. G., Treanor M., Conway C. C., Zbozinek T., Vervliet B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: an inhibitory learning approach. Behav. Res. Ther. 58, 10–23. 10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006
    1. Denniston J. C., Chang R. C., Miller R. R. (2003). Massive extinction treatment attenuates the renewal effect. Learn. Motiv. 34, 68–86. 10.1016/s0023-9690(02)00508-8
    1. Gershman S. J., Jones C. E., Norman K. A., Monfils M. H., Niv Y. (2013). Gradual extinction prevents the return of fear: implications for the discovery of state. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:164. 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00164
    1. Glotzbach E., Ewald H., Andreatta M., Pauli P., Mühlberger A. (2012). Contextual fear conditioning predicts subsequent avoidance behaviour in a virtual reality environment. Cogn. Emot. 26, 1256–1272. 10.1080/02699931.2012.656581
    1. Hamm A. O., Weike A. I. (2005). The neuropsychology of fear learning and fear regulation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 5–14. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.006
    1. Hermans D., Craske M. C., Mineka S., Lovibond P. F. (2006). Extinction in human fear conditioning. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 361–368. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.006
    1. Hofmann S. G., Smits J. A. J. (2008). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. J. Clin. Psychiatry 69, 621–632. 10.4088/jcp.v69n0415
    1. Koch M. (1999). The neurobiology of startle. Prog. Neurobiol. 59, 107–128. 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00098-7
    1. Laux L., Glanzmann P., Schaffner P., Spielberger C. (1981). Das State-Trait-Anxiety-Inventory (STAI). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz.
    1. Leaton R. N., Borszcz G. S. (1985). Potentiated startle: its relation to freezing and shock intensity in rats. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 11, 421–428. 10.1037/0097-7403.11.3.421
    1. Merikangas K. R., He J. P., Burstein M., Swanson S. A., Avenevoli S., Cui L. H., et al. . (2010). Lifetime Prevalence of Mental Disorders in U.S. Adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication-Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, 980–989. 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017
    1. Myers K. M., Davis M. (2007). Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol. Psychiatry 12, 120–150. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001939
    1. Pavlov I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. London: Oxford Univ. Press.
    1. Redish A. D., Jensen S., Johnson A., Kurth-Nelson Z. (2007). Reconciling reinforcement learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: implications for addiction, relapse and problem gambling. Psychol. Rev. 114, 784–805. 10.1037/0033-295x.114.3.784.supp
    1. Rescorla R. A., Wagner A. R. (1972). “A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement,” in Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, eds Black A. H., Prokasy W. F. (New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; ), 64–99.
    1. Rinck M., Bundschuh S., Engler S., Müller A., Wissmann J., Ellwart T., et al. (2002). Reliabilität und validität dreier instrumente zur messung von angst vor spinnen. Diagn. 48, 141–149. 10.1026/0012-1924.48.3.141
    1. Schiller D., Monfils M. H., Raio C. M., Johnson D. C., Ledoux J. E., Phelps E. A. (2010). Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature 463, 49–53. 10.1038/nature08637
    1. Shiban Y. (2013). Attenuating Renewal Following Exposure Therapy: Mechanisms of Exposure in Multiple Contexts and its Influence on the Renewal of Fear: Studies in Virtual Reality. Doctoral thesis, Universität Würzburg, Philosophische Fakultät II, Würzburg. Availiable online at:
    1. Shiban Y., Pauli P., Mühlberger A. (2013). Effect of multiple context exposure on renewal in spider phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 68–74. 10.1016/j.brat.2012.10.007
    1. Spielberger C., Gorsuch R., Lushene R. (1970). Manual for the State-Trait Inventory. California: Consulting Psychologists, Palo Alto.
    1. Szymanski J., O’Donohue W. (1995). Fear of spiders questionnaire. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 26, 31–34. 10.1016/0005-7916(94)00072-t
    1. Vervliet B., Craske M. G., Hermans D. (2013). Fear extinction and relapse: state of the art. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 9, 215–248. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542

Source: PubMed

3
Subskrybuj