Heuristic decision making in medicine

Julian N Marewski, Gerd Gigerenzer, Julian N Marewski, Gerd Gigerenzer

Abstract

Can less information be more helpful when it comes to making medical decisions? Contrary to the common intuition that more information is always better, the use of heuristics can help both physicians and patients to make sound decisions. Heuristics are simple decision strategies that ignore part of the available information, basing decisions on only a few relevant predictors. We discuss: (i) how doctors and patients use heuristics; and (ii) when heuristics outperform information-greedy methods, such as regressions in medical diagnosis. Furthermore, we outline those features of heuristics that make them useful in health care settings. These features include their surprising accuracy, transparency, and wide accessibility, as well as the low costs and little time required to employ them. We close by explaining one of the statistical reasons why heuristics are accurate, and by pointing to psychiatry as one area for future research on heuristics in health care.

Keywords: biases; bounded rationality; decision aids; ecological rationality; fast-and-frugal heuristics; medical decision making.

Figures

Figure 1.. A simple heuristic for deciding…
Figure 1.. A simple heuristic for deciding whether a patient should be assigned to the coronary care unit or to a regular nursing bed. If there is a certain anomaly in the electrocardiogram (the so-called ST segment) the patient is immediately sent to the coronary care unit. Otherwise a second predictor is considered, namely whether the patient's chief complaint is chest pain. If not, a third question is asked. This third question is a composite one: whether any of five other predictors is present. This type of heuristic is also called a fast-and-frugal tree. Fast-and-frugal trees assume that decision makers follow a series of sequential steps prior to reaching a decision. Abbreviations: NTG, nitroglycerin; Ml, myocardial infarction; T, T-waves with peaking or inversion. Adapted from ref 58 (based on Green and Mehr)1: Gigerenzer G. Gut feelings: the intelligence of the Unconscious. New York, NY: Viking Press; 2007. Copyright © Viking Press 2007
Figure 2.. The performance of a decision…
Figure 2.. The performance of a decision tree for coronary care unit allocations, compared with that of the Heart Disease Predictive Instrument, and physicians' judgments. The x-axis represents the proportion of patients who were incorrectly assigned to the coronary care unit (false positive rate), and the y-axis shows the proportion of patients who were correctly assigned to the coronary care unit (sensitivity). The diagonal line represents chance level, the area to the left of the diagonal better-than-chance. Note that the Heart Disease Predictive Instrument's allocation decisions depend on how sensitivity is traded off against the false-positive rate. This is why several data points are shown for this instrument. Adapted from ref 58 (based on Green and Mehr)': Gigerenzer G. Gut feelings: the Intelligence of the Unconscious. New York, NY: Viking Press; 2007. Copyright © Viking Press 2007
Figure 3.. A simple tree to represent…
Figure 3.. A simple tree to represent probabilities as natural frequencies, designed to help pyhsicians and patients understand health statistics.
Figure 4.. A fast-and-frugal tree for making…
Figure 4.. A fast-and-frugal tree for making decisions about macrolide prescriptions, proposed by Fisher et al (see also Katsikopoulos et al. for an in-depth discussion). Macrolides are the first-line antibiotic treatment of community-acquired pneumonia. The fast-and-frugal tree signals that first-line macrolide treatment may be limited to individuals with community-acquired pneumonia who have had fever for more than 2 days and who are older than 3 years.
Figure 5.. Illustration of how two models…
Figure 5.. Illustration of how two models fit past observations (filled circles) and how they predict new obsen/ations (triangles). The complex Model A (thin line) overfits the past observations and is not as accurate in predicting the new observations as the simple Model B (thick line Adapted from ref 60: Pitt MA, Myung IJ, Zhang S. Toward a method for selecting among computational models for cognition. Psychol Rev. 2002;109:472-491. Copyright © American Psychological Association 2002

References

    1. Green L., Mehr DR. What alters physicians' decisions to admit to the coronary care unit? J. Earn Pract. 1997;45:219–226.
    1. Edwards W. The theory of decision making. Psychol Bull. 1954;5:380–417.
    1. Kahneman D., Slovic P., Tversky A., eds. Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 1982
    1. Simon HA. Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychol Rev. 1956;63:129–138.
    1. Simon HA. Invariants of human behavior. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41:119.
    1. Gigerenzer G., Todd PM., the ABC Research Group. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 1999
    1. Marewski JN., Gaissmaier W., Gigerenzer G. Good judgments do not require complex cognition. Cogn Process. 2010;11:103–121.
    1. Hammond KR. Human Judgment and Social Policy: Irreducible Uncertainty inevitable Error, Unavoidable Injustice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. 1996
    1. Merenstein D. Winners and losers. JAMA. 2004;7:15–16.
    1. Studdert DM., Mello MM., Sage WM., et al. Defensive medicine among high-risk specialist physicians in a volatile malpractice environment. JAMA. 2005;293:2609–2617.
    1. Marewski JN., Schooler LJ. Cognitive niches: an ecological model of strategy selection. Psychol Rev. 2011;118:393–437.
    1. Rieskamp J., Otto PE. SSL: A theory of how people learn to select strategies. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2006;135:207–236.
    1. Steurer J., Held U., Schmidt M., Gigerenzer G., Tag B., Bachmann LM. Legal concerns trigger prostate-specific antigen testing. J Eval Clin Pract. 2009;15:390–392.
    1. Gigerenzer G., Engel C. Heuristics and the Law. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2006
    1. Gigerenzer G., Hertwig R., Pachur T., eds. Heuristics: the Foundations of Adaptive Behavior. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2011
    1. Todd PM., Miller GF. From pride and prejudice to persuasion: realistic heuristics for mate search. In: Gigerenzer G, Todd PM, the ABC Research Group, eds. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1999:287–308.
    1. Marewski JN., Gaissmaier W., Schooler LJ., Goldstein DG., Gigerenzer G. From recognition to decisions: extending and testing recognition-based models for multi-alternative inference. Psychon Bull Rev. 2010;17:287–309.
    1. Brandstatter E., Gigerenzer G., Hertwig R. The priority heuristic: making choices without trade-offs. Psychol Rev. 2006;113:409–432.
    1. Ortmann A., Gigerenzer G., Borges B., Goldstein DG. The recognition heuristic: a fast and frugal way to investment choice? In: Plott CR, Smith VL, eds. Handbook of Experimental Economics Results. Volume 1. Amsterdam, NL: North Holland. 2008:993–1003.
    1. Pachur T., Biele G. Forecasting from ignorance: the use and usefulness of recognition in lay predictions of sports events. Acta Psychol. 2007;125:99–116.
    1. Gaissmaier W., Marewski JN. Forecasting elections with mere recognition from lousy samples. Judgm Decis Mak. 2011;6:73–88.
    1. Wegwarth O., Gaissmaier W., Gigerenzer G. Smart strategies for doctors and doctors in training: heuristics in medicine. MedEduc. 2009;43:721–722.
    1. Croskerry P. A universal model of diagnostic reasoning. Acad Med. 2009;84:1022–1028.
    1. Elstein AS. Heuristics and biases: selected errors in clinical reasoning. Acad Med. 1999;74:791–794.
    1. McDonald C. Medical heuristics the silent adjudicators of clinical practice. Ann Intern Med. 1996;124:56–62.
    1. Elwyn G., Edwards A., Eccles M., Rovner D. Decision analysis in patient care. Lancet. 2001;358:571–574.
    1. Dharni MK., Harries C. Fast and frugal versus regression models of human judgment. Think Reasoning. 2001;7:5–27.
    1. Backlund LG., Bring J., Skaner Y., Strender LE., Montgomery H. Improving fast and frugal in relation to regression analysis: test of 3 models for medical decision making. Med Decis Mak. 2009;29:140–148.
    1. Smith L., Gilhooly K. Regression versus fast and frugal models of decision-making: the case of prescribing for depression. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2006;20:265–274.
    1. Gigerenzer G. Calculated Risks: How to Know Vvhen Numbers Deceive You. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. 2002
    1. Dhami MK. Psychological models of professional decision making. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:175–180.
    1. Berg N., Biele G., Gigerenzer G. Does consistency predict accuracy of beliefs?: economists surveyed about PSA. MPRA Paper 26590, University Library of Munich, Germany; 2010. Available at: Accessed February 2012.
    1. Die Zahl der Organspenden erhohen -Zu einem drangenden Problem der Transplantationsmedizin in Deutschland Available at http// . Berlin, Germany Nationaler Ethikrat. Accessed September 21. 2011
    1. Johnson EJ., Goldstein DG. Do defaults save lives. Science. 2003;302:1338–1339.
    1. Brighton H. Robust inference with simple cognitive models. In: Lebiere C, Wray B, eds. Between a rock and a hard place: cognitive science principles meet Al-hard problems. Papers from the AAAI spring symposium (AAAI Tech. Rep. No. SS-06-03). Menlo Park, Calif: AAAI Press. 2006:17–22.
    1. Gigerenzer G., Brighton H. Homo heuristicus:why biased minds make better inferences. Top Cogn Sci. 2009;1:107–143.
    1. Breiman L., Friedman JH., Olshen RA., Stone CJ. Classification and Regression Trees. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth International Group. 1984
    1. Quinlan JR. C4.5: Programs For Machine Learning. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann. 1993
    1. Gigerenzer G., Goldstein DG. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. Psychol Rev. 1996;104:650–669.
    1. Jenny M., Pachur T. Can a fast and frugal tree predict depression? Poster presented at: Annual Meeting of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making; November 2009; Boston. MA
    1. Gigerenzer G., Gaissmaier W., Kurz-Milcke E., Schwartz LM., Woloshin S. Helping doctors and patients make sense of health statistics. Psychol Sci Public interest. 2007;8:53–96.
    1. Muhlhauser I., Kasper J., Meyer G. FEND Understanding of diabetes prevention studies: questionnaire survey of professionals in diabetes care. Diabetologia. 2006;49:1742–1746.
    1. Gigerenzer G., Hoffrage U., Ebert A. AIDS counselling for low-risk clients. AIDS Care. 1998;10:197– 211.
    1. Casscells W., Schoenberger A., Grayboys T. Interpretation by physicians of clinical laboratory results. N Engl J Med. 1978;299:999–1001.
    1. Eddy DM. Probabilistic reasoning in clinical medicine: problems and opportunities. In: Kahneman D, Slovic P, Tversky A, eds. Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 1982:249–267.
    1. Ghosh AK., Ghosh K. Translating evidence-based information into effective risk communication: current challenges and opportunities. J Lab Clin Med. 2005;145:171–180.
    1. Hoffrage U., Lindsey S., Hertwig R., Gigerenzer G. Communicating statistical information. Science. 2000;290:2261–2262.
    1. Young JM., Glasziou P., Ward JE. General practitioners' self rating of skills in evidence based medicine: a validation study. BMJ. 2002;324:950–951.
    1. Cook L. The World Trade Center attack. The paramedic response: an insider's view. Git Care. 2001;5:301–303.
    1. Gigerenzer G., Gaissmaier W. Heuristic decision making. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62:451–482.
    1. Fischer JE., Steiner F., Zucol F., et al. Use of simple heuristics to target macrolide prescription in children with community-acquired pneumonia. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156:1005–1008.
    1. Kattah JC., Talkad AV., Wang DZ., Hsieh YH., Newman-Toker DE. HINTS to diagnose stroke in the acute vestibular syndrome. Stroke. 2009;40:3504–3510.
    1. Martignon L., Hoffrage U. Why does one-reason decision making work? A case study in ecological rationality. In: Gigerenzer G, Todd PM, The ABC Research Group, eds. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart. New York; NY: Oxford University Press. 1999:119–140.
    1. Volz KG., Schooler LJ., Schubotz Rl., Raab M., Gigerenzer G., von Cramon DY. Why you think Milan is larger than Modena: neural correlates of the recognition heuristic. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1924–1936.
    1. Glockner A., Moritz S. A fine-grained analysis of the jumping-toconclusions bias in schizophrenia: data-gathering, response confidence, and information integration. Judgm Decis Mak. 2009;4:587–600.
    1. Pachur T., Mata R., Schooler LJ. Cognitive aging and the adaptive use of recognition in decision making. Psychol Aging . 2009;24:901–915.
    1. Mata R., Schooler LJ., Rieskamp J. The aging decision maker: cognitive aging and the adaptive selection of decision strategies. Psychol Aging. 2007;22:796–810.
    1. Gigerenzer G. Gut feelings: the Intelligence of the Unconscious. New York, NY: Viking Press. 2007
    1. Katsikopoulos K., Pachur T., Machery E., Wallin A. From Meehl (1954) to fast and frugal heuristics (and back): new insights into how to bridge the clinical-actuarial divide. Theory Psychol. 2008;18:443–464.
    1. Pitt MA., Myung IJ., Zhang S. Toward a method for selecting among computational models for cognition. Psychol Rev. 2002;109:472–491.
    1. Lopes LL. Three misleading assumptions in the customary rhetoric of the bias literature. Theory Psychol. 1992;2:231–236.
    1. Marewski JN., Schooler LJ., Gigerenzer G. Five principles for studying people's use of heuristics. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 2010;42:72–87.
    1. Arzteblatt News. Available at: news/news. asp?id=44490. Accessed September 21. 2011
    1. Marewski JN., Olsson H. Beyond the null ritual: formal modeling of psychological processes. Z Psychol/J Psychol. 2009;217:49–60.

Source: PubMed

3
Subskrybuj