Practice-oriented retest learning as the basic form of cognitive plasticity of the aging brain

Lixia Yang, Lixia Yang

Abstract

It has been well documented that aging is associated with declines in a variety of cognitive functions. A growing body of research shows that the age-related cognitive declines are reversible through cognitive training programs, suggesting maintained cognitive plasticity of the aging brain. Retest learning represents a basic form of cognitive plasticity. It has been consistently demonstrated for adults in young-old and old-old ages. Accumulated research indicates that retest learning is effective, robust, endurable and could occur at a more conceptual level beyond item-specific memorization. Recent studies also demonstrate promisingly broader transfer effects from retest practice of activities involving complex executive functioning to other untrained tasks. The results shed light on the development of self-guided mental exercise programs to improve cognitive performance and efficiency of the aging brain. The relevant studies were reviewed, and the findings were discussed in light of their limitations, implications, and future directions.

References

    1. Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review. 1996;103(3):403–428.
    1. Schaie KW. Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press; 2005.
    1. Bäckman L, MacDonald SWS. Death and cognition: synthesis and outlook. European Psychologist. 2006;11(3):224–235.
    1. Baltes PB, Mayer KU. The Berlin Aging Study: Aging from 70 to 100. New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press; 1999.
    1. Rönnlund M, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Nilsson L. Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20(1):3–18.
    1. Wilson RS, Beck TL, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Terminal cognitive decline: accelerated loss of cognition in the last years of life. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2007;69(2):131–137.
    1. Hertzog C, Kramer AF, Wilson RS, Lindenberger U. Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, Supplement. 2008;9(1):1–65.
    1. Lövdén M, Bäckman L, Lindenberger U, Schaefer S, Schmiedek F. A theoretical framework for the study of adult cognitive plasticity. Psychological Bulletin. 2010;136(4):659–676.
    1. Stine-Morrow EAL, Basak C. Cognitive interventions. In: Schaie KW, Willis SL, editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 7th edition. San Diego, Calif, USA: Elsevier Academic Press; 2011. pp. 153–171.
    1. Thompson G, Foth D. Cognitive-training programs for older adults: what are they and can they enhance mental fitness? Educational Gerontology. 2005;31(8):603–626.
    1. Verhaeghen P, Marcoen A, Goossens L. Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. Psychology and aging. 1992;7(2):242–251.
    1. Schaie KW, Willis SL. Can decline in adult intellectual functioning be reversed? Developmental Psychology. 1986;22(2):223–232.
    1. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288(18):2271–2281.
    1. Baltes PB, Willis SL. Plasticity and enhancement of intellectual functioning in old age: Penn State's adult development and enrichment project (ADEPT) In: Craik FIM, Trehub EE, editors. Aging and Cognitive Processes. New York, NY, USA: Plenum Press; 1982. pp. 353–389.
    1. Craik FIM, Winocur G, Palmer H, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: effects on memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2007;13(1):132–142.
    1. Stuss DT, Robertson IH, Craik FIM, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: a randomized trial to evaluate a new protocol. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2007;13(1):120–131.
    1. Baltes PB, Linderberger U. On the range of cognitive plasticity in old age as a function of experience: 15 Years of intervention research. Behavior Therapy. 1988;19(3):283–300.
    1. Rebok GW, Carlson MC, Langbaum JB. Training and maintaining memory abilities in healthy older adults: traditional and novel approaches. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2007;62B(I):53–61.
    1. Baltes PB, Sowarka D, Kliegl R. Cognitive training research on fluid intelligence in old age: what can older adults achieve by themselves? Psychology and aging. 1989;4(2):217–221.
    1. Singer T, Lindenberger U, Baltes PB. Plasticity of memory for new learning in very old age: a story of major loss? Psychology and Aging. 2003;18(2):306–317.
    1. Bherer L, Kramer AF, Peterson MS, Colcombe S, Erickson K, Becic E. Training effects on dual-task performance: are there age-related differences in plasticity of attentional control? Psychology and Aging. 2005;20(4):695–709.
    1. Li S, Schmiedek F, Huxhold O, Röcke C, Smith J, Lindenberger U. Working memory plasticity in old age: practice gain, transfer, and maintenance. Psychology and Aging. 2008;23(4):731–742.
    1. Yang L, Krampe RT, Baltes PB. Basic forms of cognitive plasticity extended into the oldest-old: retest learning, age, and cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21(2):372–378.
    1. Karbach J, Kray J. How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Developmental Science. 2009;12(6):978–990.
    1. Willis SL. Methodological issues in behavioral intervention research with the elderly. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW, editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 5th edition. San Diego, Calif, USA: Academic Press; 2001. pp. 78–108.
    1. Kramer AF, Willis SL. Enhancing the cognitive vitality of older adults. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11(5):173–177.
    1. Greenwood PM. Functional plasticity in cognitive aging: review and hypothesis. Neuropsychology. 2007;21(6):657–673.
    1. Goh JO, Park DC. Neuroplasticity and cognitive aging: the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience. 2009;27(5):391–403.
    1. Ferrer E, Salthouse TA, Stewart WF, Schwartz BS. Modeling age and retest processes in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. Psychology and Aging. 2004;19(2):243–259.
    1. Ferrer E, Salthouse TA, McArdle JJ, Stewart WF, Schwartz BS. Multivariate modeling of age and retest in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20(3):412–422.
    1. Salthouse TA, Schroeder DH, Ferrer E. Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40(5):813–822.
    1. Salthouse TA. Influence of age on practice effects in longitudinal neurocognitive change. Neuropsychology. 2010;24(5):563–572.
    1. Hofland BF, Willis SL, Baltes PB. Fluid intelligence performance in the elderly: intraindividual variability and conditions of assessment. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1981;73(4):573–586.
    1. Hoyer WJ, Labouvie GV, Baltes PB. Modification of response speed deficits and intellectual performance in the elderly. Human Development. 1973;16(3):233–242.
    1. Dahlin E, Neely AS, Larsson A, Bäckman L, Nyberg L. Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science. 2008;320(5882):1510–1512.
    1. Kramer AF, Larish JF, Strayer DL. Training for attentional control in dual task settings: a comparison of young and old adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 1995;1(1):50–76.
    1. Wilkinson A, Yang L. Plasticity of inhibition in older adults: retest practice and transfer effects. Psychology and Aging. In press.
    1. Erickson KI, Colcombe SJ, Wadhwa R, et al. Training-induced functional activation changes in dual-task processing: an fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17(1):192–204.
    1. Buschkuehl M, Jaeggi SM, Hutchison S, et al. Impact of working memory training on memory performance in old-old adults. Psychology and Aging. 2008;23(4):743–753.
    1. Salthouse TA, Tucker-Drob E. Implications of short-term retest effects for the interpretation of longitudinal change. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(6):800–811.
    1. Yang L, Reed M, Russo FA, Wilkinson A. A new look at retest learning in older adults: learning in the absence of item-specific effects. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2009;64B(4):470–473.
    1. Saczynski JS, Willis SL, Schaie KW. Strategy use in reasoning training with older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 2002;9(1):48–60.
    1. Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, et al. Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA. 2006;296(23):2805–2814.
    1. Willis SL, Nesselroade CS. Long-term effects of fluid ability training in old-old age. Developmental Psychology. 1990;26(6):905–910.
    1. Yang L, Krampe RT. Long-term maintenance of retest learning in young old and oldest old adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2009;64B(5):608–611.
    1. Wilkinson A, Yang L. Plasticity of inhibition in older adults: 1-year maintenance of retest practice effects . In preparation.
    1. Owen AM, Hampshire A, Grahn JA, et al. Putting brain training to the test. Nature. 2010;465(7299):775–778.
    1. Basak C, Boot WR, Voss MW, Kramer AF. Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychology and Aging. 2008;23(4):765–777.
    1. Carlson MC, Saczynski JS, Rebok GW, et al. Exploring the effects of an "everyday" activity program on executive function and memory in older adults: Experience corps® . The Gerontologist. 2008;48(6):793–801.
    1. Stine-Morrow EAL, Parisi JM, Morrow DG, Park DC. The effects of an engaged lifestyle on cognitive vitality: a field experiment. Psychology and Aging. 2008;23(4):778–786.
    1. Tranter LJ, Koutstaal W. Age and flexible thinking: an experimental demonstration of the beneficial effects of increased cognitively stimulating activity on fluid intelligence in healthy older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 2008;15(2):184–207.
    1. Smith GE, Housen P, Yaffe K, et al. A cognitive training program based on principles of brain plasticity: results from the improvement in memory with plasticity-based adaptive cognitive training (IMPACT) study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2009;57(4):594–603.
    1. West RL. An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin. 1996;120(2):272–292.
    1. Raz N. The aging brain observed in vivo: differential changes and their modifiers. In: Cabeza R, Nyberg L, Park DC, editors. Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging: Linking Cognitive and Cerebral Aging. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press; 2004. pp. 19–57.
    1. Schmiedek F, Bauer C, Lövdén M, Brose A, Lindenberger U. Cognitive enrichment in old age: web-based training programs. GeroPsych. 2010;23(2):59–67.

Source: PubMed

3
Subskrybuj