A network approach to psychopathology: new insights into clinical longitudinal data

Laura F Bringmann, Nathalie Vissers, Marieke Wichers, Nicole Geschwind, Peter Kuppens, Frenk Peeters, Denny Borsboom, Francis Tuerlinckx, Laura F Bringmann, Nathalie Vissers, Marieke Wichers, Nicole Geschwind, Peter Kuppens, Frenk Peeters, Denny Borsboom, Francis Tuerlinckx

Abstract

In the network approach to psychopathology, disorders are conceptualized as networks of mutually interacting symptoms (e.g., depressed mood) and transdiagnostic factors (e.g., rumination). This suggests that it is necessary to study how symptoms dynamically interact over time in a network architecture. In the present paper, we show how such an architecture can be constructed on the basis of time-series data obtained through Experience Sampling Methodology (ESM). The proposed methodology determines the parameters for the interaction between nodes in the network by estimating a multilevel vector autoregression (VAR) model on the data. The methodology allows combining between-subject and within-subject information in a multilevel framework. The resulting network architecture can subsequently be analyzed through network analysis techniques. In the present study, we apply the method to a set of items that assess mood-related factors. We show that the analysis generates a plausible and replicable network architecture, the structure of which is related to variables such as neuroticism; that is, for subjects who score high on neuroticism, worrying plays a more central role in the network. Implications and extensions of the methodology are discussed.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Estimated population network at baseline.
Figure 1. Estimated population network at baseline.
The six items are: C = cheerful, E = pleasant event, W = worry, F = fearful, S = sad and R = relaxed. Solid green arrows correspond to positive connections and red dashed arrows to negative connections. Only arrows that surpass the significance threshold are shown (i.e., for which the p-value of the t-statistic is smaller than 0.05). Arrows can be either red, indicating a negative relationship (i.e., ), or green, indicating a positive relationship (i.e., ). Furthermore, the strength of the relation from item k to item j (i.e., an extremer value for ) is translated into the thickness of the arrows: the thicker the arrow between two nodes, the stronger the nodes or items are related. Note that item responses can also be predicted from the previous state of the item itself. These arrows are the self-loops in the network.
Figure 2. Inter-individual differences of the arrows…
Figure 2. Inter-individual differences of the arrows of the network from Figure 1.
The thickness of the arrows is based on the size of the standard deviation of the random effects. To construct the figure, we have put a cutoff of 0.1 on the standard deviation and only the standard deviations above the cutoff are shown with a non-transparent arrow. As the threshold for the standard deviation of the random effects 0.1 was chosen because it represents large inter-individual differences. The average coefficient of the self-loops (i.e., autoregression coefficients) is about 0.2 with a random effects standard deviation of 0.1. Therefore, assuming a normal distribution, the range from 0 to 0.4 represents 95% of the individual self-loop coefficients. With a larger cutoff, such as 0.2, also individuals having negative self-loops would be taken into account. However, more than 95% of the population has a positive self-loop strength.
Figure 3. Individual networks (at baseline) of…
Figure 3. Individual networks (at baseline) of two different persons.
Figure 4. Centrality (betweenness) of each item…
Figure 4. Centrality (betweenness) of each item in the network as a function of level of neuroticism at baseline.
Low, mid, and high neuroticism are shown from left to right. The labels of the items are abbreviated by their first letter (C = cheerful, S = sad, R = relaxed, W = worry, F = fearful and E = event). The black dots are the model-based estimate of betweenness, the darkgrey vertical lines represent 50% confidence intervals and the light grey vertical lines represent 95% confidence intervals (as estimated from the bootstrap method). Together, the median, 50% and 95% confidence intervals give information on how the node centrality for every item in all three networks is distributed.
Figure 5. Correspondence between the basis dataset…
Figure 5. Correspondence between the basis dataset and the validation dataset.
Left panel: Representation of the correspondence between the population network coefficients (fixed effects) of the basis dataset (x-axis) and the validation dataset (y-axis). Right panel: Representation of the correspondence between the inter-individual differences (random effects) of the basis data (x-axis) and the validation data (y-axis).
Figure 6. Centrality (betweenness) of each item…
Figure 6. Centrality (betweenness) of each item in the network as a function of level of neuroticism in the validation dataset.
Low, mid, and high neuroticism are shown from left to right. The labels of the items are abbreviated by their first letter (C = cheerful, S = sad, R = relaxed, W = worry and F = fearful). The black dots are the model-based estimate of betweenness, the darkgrey vertical lines represent 50% confidence intervals and the light grey vertical lines represent 95% confidence intervals (as estimated from the bootstrap method). Together, the median, 50% and 95% confidence intervals give information on how the node centrality for every item in all three networks is distributed.

References

    1. Borsboom D (2008) Psychometric perspectives on diagnostic systems. J Clin Psychol 64: 1089–1108 doi:
    1. Borsboom D, Cramer AOJ, Schmittmann VD, Epskamp S, Waldorp LJ (2011) The small world of psychopathology. PLoS One 6: e27407 doi:
    1. Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, van der Maas HLJ, Borsboom D (2010) Comorbidity: A network perspective. Behav Brain Sci 33: 137–193 doi:
    1. Cramer AOJ, Borsboom D, Aggen SH, Kendler KS (2012) The pathoplasticity of dysphoric episodes: Differential impact of stressful life events on the pattern of depressive symptom inter-correlations. Psychol Med 42: 957–967 doi:
    1. Kendler KS (2012) Levels of explanation in psychiatric and substance use disorders: Implications for the development of an etiologically based nosology. Mol Psychiatry 17: 11–21 doi:
    1. Schmittmann VD, Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, Epskamp S, Kievit RA, et al... (2011) Deconstructing the construct: A network perspective on psychological phenomena. New Ideas Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.02.007.
    1. Kendler KS, Zachar P, Craver C (2011) What kinds of things are psychiatric disorders? Psychol Med 41: 1143–1150 doi:
    1. Ebner-Priemer UW, Eid M, Kleindienst N, Stabenow S, Trull TJ (2009) Analytic strategies for understanding affective (in)stability and other dynamic processes in psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol 118: 195–202 doi:
    1. Kuppens P, Oravecz Z, Tuerlinckx F (2010) Feelings change: Accounting for individual differences in the temporal dynamics of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 99: 1042–1060 doi:
    1. Meney I, Waterhouse J, Atkinson G, Reilly T, Davenne D (1998) The effect of one night’s sleep deprivation on temperature, mood, and physical performance in subjects with different amounts of habitual physical activity. Chronobiol Int 14: 125–32.
    1. Geschwind N, Peeters F, Drukker M, van Os J, Wichers M (2011) Mindfulness training increases momentary positive emotions and reward experience in adults vulnerable to depression: A randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 79: 618–628 doi:
    1. Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R (1987) Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. J Nerv Ment Dis 175: 526–536.
    1. Stone AA, Shiffman SS (1994) Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavioral medicine. Ann Behav Med 16: 199–202.
    1. Bolger N, Davis A, Rafaeli E (2003) Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annu Rev Psychol 54: 579–616 doi:
    1. Hendrickx DM, Hendriks MMWB, Eilers PHC, Smilde AK, Hoefsloot HCJ (2011) Reverse engineering of metabolic networks: A critical assessment. Mol Biosyst 7: 511–520 doi:
    1. Sporns O (2011) Networks of the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    1. Gates KM, Molenaar PC (2012) Group search algorithm recovers effective connectivity maps for individuals in homogeneous and heterogeneous samples. Neuroimage 63: 310–319 doi:
    1. Hamilton JD (1994) Time Series Analysis. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
    1. Shumway RS, Stoffer DS (2006) Time series analysis and its applications: With R examples. New York: Springer.
    1. Schwartz JE, Stone AA (1998) Strategies for analysing ecological momentary assessment data. Health Psychol 17: 6–16.
    1. The R project for statistical computing. Available: . Accessed 2012 Dec 12.
    1. Barrett LF (1998) Discrete emotions or dimensions? The role of valence focus and arousal focus. Cogn Emot 12: 579–599.
    1. Reisenzein R (1994) Pleasure-arousal theory and the intensity of emotions. J Pers Soc Psychol 67: 525–539.
    1. Russell JA (1980) A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol 39: 1161–1178.
    1. Russell JA, Weiss A, Mendelsohn GA (1989) Affect grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. J Pers Soc Psychol 57: 493–502.
    1. Smith CA, Ellsworth PC (1985) Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. J Pers Soc Psychol 48: 813–838.
    1. Baas M, De Dreu CKW, Nijstad BA (2008) A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychol Bull 134: 779–806 doi:
    1. Larsen RJ, Diener E (1992) Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. In: Clark MS, editor. Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 13): Emotion. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 25–59.
    1. Watson D, Tellegen A (1985) Toward a consensual structure of mood. Psychol Bull 98: 219–235.
    1. Brosschot JF, Gerin W, Thayer JF (2006) The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. J Psychosom Res 60: 113–124 doi:
    1. Borkovec TD, Ray WJ, Stöber J (1998) Worry: A cognitive phenomenon intimately linked to affective, physiological, and interpersonal behavioral processes. Cognit Ther Res 22: 561–576.
    1. Gruber J, Eidelman P, Harvey AG (2008) Transdiagnostic emotion regulation processes in bipolar disorder and insomnia. Behav Res Ther 46: 1096–1100 doi:
    1. Pfaff B (2008) Analysis of Integrated and Cointegrated Time Series with R. New York: Springer.
    1. Snijders T, Bosker R (2012) Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage Publications.
    1. Walls TA, Schafer JL (2006) Models for Intensive Longitudinal Data. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    1. Lodewyckx T, Tuerlinckx F, Kuppens P, Allen NB, Sheeber LB (2011) A hierarchical state space approach to affective dynamics. J Math Psychol 55: 68–83 doi:
    1. Oravecz Z, Vandekerckhove J, Tuerlinckx F (2011) A hierarchical latent stochastic differential equation model for affective dynamics. Psychol Methods 16: 468–490 doi:
    1. Pe ML, Kuppens P (2012) The dynamic interplay between emotions in daily life: Augmentation, blunting, and the role of appraisal overlap. Emotion 12: 1320–1328 doi:
    1. Tschacher W, Ramseyer F (2009) Modeling psychotherapy process by time-series panel analysis (TSPA). Psychother Res 19: 469–481 doi:
    1. Tschacher W, Zorn P, Ramseyer F (2012) Change Mechanisms of Schema-Centered Group Psychotherapy with Personality Disorder Patients. PLoS ONE 7: e39687 doi:
    1. Schmid CH (2001) Marginal and dynamic regression models for longitudinal data. Stat Med 20: 3295–3311 doi:
    1. Funatogawa I, Funatogawa T, Ohashi Y (2007) An autoregressive linear mixed effects model for the analysis of longitudinal data which show profiles approaching asymptotes. Stat Med 26: 2113–2130 doi:
    1. Horváth C, Wieringa JE (2008) Pooling data for the analysis of dynamic marketing systems. Stat Neerl 62: 208–229 doi:
    1. Boccaletti S, Latora V, Moreno Y, Chavez M, Hwang DU (2006) Complex networks: Structure and dynamics. Phys Rep 424: 175–308 doi:
    1. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol 57: 289–300.
    1. Epskamp S, Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, Schmittmann VD, Borsboom D (2012) Qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J Stat Softw 48: 1–18.
    1. Arnold B, Strauss D (1991) Pseudolikelihood estimation: Some examples. Sankhya Ser B 53: 233–243.
    1. Fieuws S, Verbeke G (2006) Pairwise fitting of mixed models for the joint modeling of multivariate longitudinal profiles. Biometrics 62: 424–431 doi:
    1. Bates DM, Maechler M, Bolker B (2011) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999999-0.
    1. Verbeke G, Molenberghs G (2000) Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New York: Springer.
    1. Baltagi BH (2005) Economotric analysis of panel data. Chichester: Wiley.
    1. Verbeke G, Spiessens B, Lesaffre E (2001) Conditional linear mixed models. Am Stat 55: 25–34 doi:
    1. Neyman J, Scott E (1948) Consistent estimates based on partially consistent observations. Econometrica 16: 1–32.
    1. Gelman A, Hill J (2007) Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierachical Models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    1. Tuerlinckx F, Rijmen F, Verbeke G, De Boeck P (2006) Statistical inference in generalized linear mixed models: A review. Br J Math Stat Psychol 59: 225–255 doi:
    1. Opsahl T, Agneessens F, Skvoretz F (2010) Node centrality in weighted networks: Generalizing degree and shortest paths. Soc Networks 32: 245–251 doi:
    1. Hoekstra HA, Ormel J, De Fruyt F (1996) NEO PI-R. NEO FFI. Big Five Persoonlijkheidsvragenlijsten: Handleiding [NEO PI-R. NEO FFI. Big Five Personality questionaires: Manual]. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.
    1. Efron B, Tibshirani R (1994) An Introduction to the Bootstrap. New York: Chapman & Hall.
    1. Pe ML, Raes F, Koval P, Brans K, Verduyn P, et al... (2012) Interference resolution moderates the impact of rumination and reappraisal on affective experiences in daily life. Cogn Emot. In press. doi:10.1080/02699931.2012.719489.
    1. Pe ML, Koval P, Kuppens P (2012) Executive well-being: Updating of positive stimuli in working memory is associated with subjective well-being. Cognition. In Press. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.002.
    1. Koval P, Kuppens P, Allen NB, Sheeber LB (2012) Getting stuck in depression: The roles of rumination and emotional inertia. Cogn Emot 26: 1412–1427.
    1. Gosling S, Rentfrow P, Swann W (2003) A very brief measure of the big five personality domains. J Res Pers 37: 504–528 doi:
    1. Hofmans J, Kuppens P, Allik J (2008) Is short in length short in content? An examination of the domain representation of the Ten Item Personality Inventory scales in Dutch language. Pers Individ Dif 45: 750–755 doi:
    1. Lavie P (2001) Sleep-wake as a biological rhythm. Annu Rev Psychol 52: 277–303.
    1. Box JEP, Jenkins GM, Reinsel GC (1994) Time series analysis: Forecasting and control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
    1. Kwiatkowski D, Phillips PCB, Schmidt P (1992) ShinY (1992) Testing the null hypothesis of stationarity against the alternative of a unit root. J Econom 54: 159–178.
    1. Schwarz G (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics 6: 461–64.
    1. Lütkepohl H (1993) Introduction to Multiple Time Series Analysis. Berlin: Springer.
    1. Fredrickson BL, Joiner T (2002) Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychol Sci 13: 172–75.
    1. Zelenski JM, Larsen RJ (2000) The distribution of basic emotions in everyday life: A state and trait perspective from experience sampling data. J Res Pers 34: 178–197 doi:
    1. Larsen JT, McGraw AP, Cacioppo JT (2001) Can people feel happy and sad at the same time? J Pers Soc Psychol 81: 684–696 doi:
    1. Russell JA, Carroll JM (1999) On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychol Bull 125: 3–30.
    1. Watson D, Wiese D, Vaidya J, Tellegen A (1999) The two general activation systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol 76: 820–838.
    1. McLaughlin K, Borkovec TD, Sibrava NJ (2007) The effect of worry and rumination on affective states and cognitive activity. Behav Ther 38: 23–38.
    1. Segerstrom SC, Tsao JCI, Alden LE, Craske MG (2000) Worry and rumination: Repetitive thought as a concomitant and predictor of negative mood. Cognit Ther Res 24: 671–688.
    1. Moberly NJ, Watkins ER (2008) Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: An experience sampling study. J Abnorm Psycho 117: 314–323 doi:
    1. Muris P, Roelofs J, Meesters C, Boomsma P (2004) Rumination and worry in nonclinical adolescents. Cognit Ther Res 28: 539–554.
    1. Wild B, Eichler M, Friederich H, Hartmann M, Zipfel S, et al.. (2010) A graphical vector autoregressive modelling approach to the analysis of electronic diary data. BMC Med Res Methodol 10: 28. Available: . Accessed 21 February 2012.
    1. Gather U, Imhoff M, Fried R (2002) Graphical models for multivariate time series from intensive care monitoring. Stat Med 21: 2685–2701 doi:

Source: PubMed

3
Subskrybuj