Psychosocial deprivation and receptive language ability: a two-sample study

Kathryn L Humphreys, Laura S Machlin, Katherine L Guyon-Harris, Charles A Nelson, Nathan A Fox, Charles H Zeanah, Kathryn L Humphreys, Laura S Machlin, Katherine L Guyon-Harris, Charles A Nelson, Nathan A Fox, Charles H Zeanah

Abstract

Background: The quality of early caregiving experiences is a known contributor to the quality of the language experiences young children receive. What is unknown is whether, and if so, how psychosocial deprivation early in life is associated with long-lasting receptive language outcomes.

Methods: Two prospective longitudinal studies examining early psychosocial deprivation/neglect in different contexts (i.e., deprivation due to institutional care or deprivation experienced by children residing within US families) and receptive language as assessed via the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) were used to assess the magnitude of these associations. First, 129 participants from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project, a randomized controlled trial of foster care as an alternative to institutional care in Romania, completed a receptive language assessment at age 18 years. Second, from the USA, 3342 participants from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study were assessed from infancy until middle childhood.

Results: Children exposed to early institutional care, on average, had lower receptive language scores than their never institutionalized counterparts in late adolescence. While randomization to an early foster care intervention had no long-lasting association with PPVT scores, the duration of childhood exposure to institutional care was negatively associated with receptive language. Psychosocial deprivation in US families was also negatively associated with receptive language longitudinally, and this association remained statistically significant even after accounting for measures of socioeconomic status.

Conclusion: Experiences of psychosocial deprivation may have long-lasting consequences for receptive language ability, extending to age 18 years. Psychosocial deprivation is an important prospective predictor of poorer receptive language.

Trial registration: Bucharest Early Intervention Project ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT00747396.

Keywords: Deprivation; Neglect; Receptive language; Socioeconomic status.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
PRISMA for study flow from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Childhood deprivation experiences and receptive language were assessed as part 5 of the 3rd Follow-up/age 16 year assessment (when participants were approximately age 18 years)
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Scatterplot illustrating the association between percent time in institutional care from birth to age 18 years and standard score from the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) at age 18 years. Raw associations are presented and statistics provided in the text are adjusted for participant sex
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Model demonstrating associations between deprivation and receptive language ability of the child at year 9. Coefficients are standardized betas. Solid lines depict significant paths. Cognitive stimulation at years 1 and 3 was allowed to covary. For factor loadings of deprivation, see Table 1

References

    1. Almas AN, Papp LJ, Woodbury MR, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. The impact of caregiving disruptions of previously institutionalized children on multiple outcomes in late childhood. Child Dev. 2018. 10.1111/cdev.13169.
    1. Anglin, J. M., Miller, G. A., & Wakefield, P. C. (1993). Vocabulary development: a morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(10).
    1. Beck L, Kumschick IR, Eid M, Klann-Delius G. Relationship between language competence and emotional competence in middle childhood. Emotion. 2012;12(3):503–514. 10.1037/a0026320.
    1. Behen ME, Helder E, Rothermel R, Solomon K, Chugani HT. Incidence of specific absolute neurocognitive impairment in globally intact children with histories of early severe deprivation. Child Neuropsychology. 2008;14(5):453–469. 10.1080/09297040802244136.
    1. Berens, A. E., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). The science of early adversity: is there a role for large institutions in the care of vulnerable children? Lancet, 386(9991), 388–398. 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61131-4.
    1. Beverly, B. L., McGuinness, T. M., & Blanton, D. J. (2008). Communication and academic challenges in early adolescence for children who have been adopted from the former Soviet Union. Language Speech Hearing Serv School, 39(3), 303–313. 10.1044/0161-1461(2008/029).
    1. Black, B., & Logan, A. (1995). Links between communication patterns in mother-child, father-child, and child-peer interactions and children’s social status. Child Dev, 66(1), 255–271. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00869.x.
    1. Chugani, H. T., Behen, M. E., Muzik, O., Juhász, C., Nagy, F., & Chugani, D. C. (2001). Local brain functional activity following early deprivation: a study of postinstitutionalized Romanian orphans. NeuroImage, 14(6), 1290–1301. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0917.
    1. Croft, C., Beckett, C., Rutter, M., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Groothues, C., … Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes of institutionally-deprived and non-deprived adoptees. II: language as a protective factor and a vulnerable outcome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 48(1), 31–44. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01689.x.
    1. De Bellis, M. D., Hooper, S. R., Spratt, E. G., & Woolley, D. P. (2009). Neuropsychological findings in childhood neglect and their relationships to pediatric PTSD. Neuropsychological Society, 15(6), 868–878. .
    1. Dong, M., Anda, R., & Felitti, V. (2004). The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse Negl, 28, 771–784. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.008.
    1. Dozier, M., Zeanah, C. H., Wallin, A. R., & Shauffer, C. (2012). Institutional care for young children: review of literature and policy implications. Soc Issues Policy Rev, 6(1), 1–25. 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01033.x.
    1. Duncan, G. J., Magnuson, K., & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2017). Moving beyond correlations in assessing the consequences of poverty. Annu Rev Psychol, 68(1), 413–434. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044224.
    1. Dunn LM, Dunn DM. Peabody picture vocabulary test. 1997.
    1. Dunn LM, Dunn DM. Peabody picture vocabulary test-fourth edition. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson; 2007.
    1. Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2007). Language, social behavior, and the quality of friendships in adolescents with and without a history of specific language impairment. Child Dev, 78(5), 1441–1457. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01076.x.
    1. Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Dev Sci, 16(2), 234–248. 10.1111/desc.12019.
    1. Flora, D. B., & Curran, P. J. (2004). An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychol Methods, 9(4), 466–491. 10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.466.
    1. Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., Hart, C. H., & Fitzgerald, A. H. (1999). Peer acceptance and friendship in children with specific language impairment. Top Lang Disord, 19(2), 34–48. 10.1097/00011363-199902000-00005.
    1. Goldfarb, W. (1945). Psychological privation in infancy and subsequent adjustment. Am J Orthopsychiatry, 15, 247–255. 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1945.tb04938.x.
    1. Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young american children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1995.
    1. Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. In Journal of Communication Disorders (Vol. 38, pp. 271–278). 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.02.003.
    1. Hood, M., Conlon, E., & Andrews, G. (2008). Preschool home literacy practices and children’s literacy development: a longitudinal analysis. J Educ Psychol, 100(2), 252–271. 10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.252.
    1. Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. R. (2008). Structural equation modelling: guidelines for determining model fit. Electronic J Business Res Methods, 6(1), 53–60. 10.21427/D79B73.
    1. Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model, 6(1), 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118.
    1. Humphreys KL, King LS, Gotlib IH. Neglect. In: Zeanah CH, editor. Handbook of infant mental health. 4. New York: Guilford Press; 2018. pp. 239–256.
    1. Hunt JM, Mohandessi K, Ghodssi M, Akiyama M. The psychological development of orphanage-reared infants: interventions with outcomes. Genet Psychol Monogr. 1976;94(2):177–226.
    1. Ing, C., DiMaggio, C., Whitehouse, A., Hegarty, M. K. Brady, J., von Ungern-Sternberg, B. S., Davidson, A., … Sun, L. S. (2012). Long-term differences in language and cognitive function after childhood exposure to anesthesia. Peditrics, 130(3), e476–e485. 10.1542/peds.2011-3822.
    1. King, L. S., Humphreys, K. L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2019). The neglect–enrichment continuum: characterizing variation in early caregiving environments. Dev Rev, 51, 109–122. 10.1016/j.dr.2019.01.001.
    1. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (fourth Edi) New York: The Gulliford Press; 2016.
    1. Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255(5044), 606–608. 10.1126/science.1736364.
    1. Levine LE, Munsch J. Child development: an active learning approach. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications Inc.; 2011.
    1. Loman, M. M., Wiik, K. L., Frenn, K. A., Pollak, S. D., & Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Postinstitutionalized children’s development: growth, cognitive, and language outcomes. J Dev Behav Pediatr, 30(5), 426–434. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181b1fd08.
    1. Lum, J. A. G., Powell, M., Timms, L., & Snow, P. (2015). A meta-analysis of cross sectional studies investigating language in maltreated children. J Speech Lang Hearing Res, 58, 961–976. 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0056.
    1. McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2017). Neglect as a violation of species-expectant experience: neurodevelopmental consequences. Biol Psychiatry, 82(7), 462–471. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.02.1096.
    1. Miller AB, Machlin L, McLaughlin KA, Sheridan M. Deprivation and psychopathology in the fragile families study: a 15-year longitudinal investigation. 2020.
    1. Miller, A. B., Sheridan, M. A., Hanson, J. L., McLaughlin, K. A., Bates, J. E., Lansford, J. E., … Dodge, K. A. (2018). Dimensions of deprivation and threat, psychopathology, and potential mediators: a multi-year longitudinal analysis. J Abnorm Psychol 10.1037/abn0000331.
    1. Millum, J., & Emanuel, E. J. (2007). Ethics: the ethics of international research with abandoned children. Science, 318(5858), 1874–1875. 10.1126/science.1153822.
    1. Mughal MK., Ginn CS., Perry RL., & Benzies KM. (2016). Early child development and care longitudinal effects of a two-generation preschool programme on receptive language skill in low-income Canadian children to age 10 years. Early Child Dev Care, 186(8), 1316–1326. 10.1080/03004430.2015.1092141.
    1. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2012.
    1. Nelson, C. A., Bos, K., Gunnar, M. R., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2011). V. the neurobiological toll of early human deprivation. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev, 76(4), 127–146. 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00630.x.
    1. Perkins SC, Finegood ED, Swain JE. Poverty and language development: roles of parenting and stress. Innov Clin Neuroscience. 2013;10(4):10–19.
    1. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods, 40, 879–891. 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.
    1. Provence S, Lipton RC. Infants in institutions. New York: International Univ. Press; 1962.
    1. Qi, C. H., Kaiser, A. P., Milan, S., & Hancock, T. (2006). Language performance of low-income African American and European American preschool children on the PPVT-III. Lang Speech Hearing Serv Schools, 37(1), 5–16. 10.1044/0161-1461(2006/002).
    1. Reichman, N. E., Teitler, J. O., Garfinkel, I., & McLanahan, S. S. (2001). Fragile families: sample and design. Child Youth Serv Rev, 23(4–5), 303–326. 10.1016/S0190-7409(01)00141-4.
    1. Rice, M. L., & Hoffman, L. (2015). Predicting vocabulary growth in children with and without specific language impairment: a longitudinal study from 2;6 to 21 years of age. J Speech Lang Hearing Res, 58(2), 345–359. 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0150.
    1. Ryan, A., Gibbon, F. E., & Oshea, A. (2015). Expressive and receptive language skills in preschool children from a socially disadvantaged area. Int J Speech Lang Pathol, 18(1), 41–52. 10.3109/17549507.2015.1089935.
    1. Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: a five-year longitudinal study. Child Dev, 73(2), 445–460. 10.1111/1467-8624.00417.
    1. Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M., Stothard, S. E., Chipchase, B., & Kaplan, C. (2006). Psychosocial outcomes at 15 years of children with a preschool history of speech-language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatr Allied Disciplines, 47(8), 759–765. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01631.x.
    1. Spratt, E. G., Friedenberg, S. L., Swenson, C. C., Larosa, A., De Bellis, M. D., Macias, M. M., … Brady, K. T. (2012). The effects of early neglect on cognitive, language, and behavioral functioning in childhood. Psychology, 3(2), 175–182. 10.4236/psych.2012.32026.
    1. Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Moore, D. W., & Runyan, D. (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the parent-child conflict tactics scales: development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse Negl, 22(4), 249–270. 10.1016/S0145-2134(97)00174-9.
    1. Taylor, C. L., Christensen, D., Lawrence, D., Mitrou, F., & Zubrick, S. R. (2013). Risk factors for children’s receptive vocabulary development from four to eight years in the longitudinal study of Australian children. PLoS One, 8(9). 10.1371/journal.pone.0073046.
    1. Tizard, B., Cooperman, O., Joseph, A., & Tizard, J. (1972). Environmental effects on language development: a study of young children in long-stay residential nurseries. Child Dev, 43(2), 337–358.
    1. Tomasello M. Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2003.
    1. Walker, D., Greenwood, C., Hart, B., & Carta, J. (1994). Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Dev, 65(2), 606–621. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00771.x.
    1. Windsor, J., Benigno, J. P., Wing, C. A., Carroll, P. J., Koga, S. F., Nelson, C. A., … Zeanah, C. H. (2011). Effect of foster care on young children’s language learning. Child Dev, 82(4), 1040–1046. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01604.x.
    1. Windsor, J., Moraru, A., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., & Zeanah, C. H. (2013). Effect of foster care on language learning at eight years: findings from the Bucharest early intervention project. J Child Language, 40(3), 605–627. 10.1017/S0305000912000177.
    1. Yew, S. G. K., & O’Kearney, R. (2013). Emotional and behavioural outcomes later in childhood and adolescence for children with specific language impairments: meta-analyses of controlled prospective studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatr Allied Disciplines, 54(5), 516–524. 10.1111/jcpp.12009.
    1. Yew, S. G. K., & O’Kearney, R. (2015). The role of early language difficulties in the trajectories of conduct problems across childhood. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 43(8), 1515–1527. 10.1007/s10802-015-0040-9.
    1. Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2012). The Bucharest early intervention project: case study in the ethics of mental health research. J Nerv Ment Dis, 200(3), 243–247. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318247d275.
    1. Zeanah, C. H., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., Smyke, A. T., Marshall, P., Parker, S. W., & Koga, S. (2003). Designing research to study the effects of institutionalization on brain and behavioral development: the Bucharest early intervention project. Dev Psychopathol, 15(4), 885–907. 10.1017/S0954579403000452.

Source: PubMed

3
Se inscrever