A sleep schedule incorporating naps benefits the transformation of hierarchical knowledge

Hosein Aghayan Golkashani, Ruth L F Leong, Shohreh Ghorbani, Ju Lynn Ong, Guillén Fernández, Michael W L Chee, Hosein Aghayan Golkashani, Ruth L F Leong, Shohreh Ghorbani, Ju Lynn Ong, Guillén Fernández, Michael W L Chee

Abstract

Study objectives: The learning brain establishes schemas (knowledge structures) that benefit subsequent learning. We investigated how sleep and having a schema might benefit initial learning followed by rearranged and expanded memoranda. We concurrently examined the contributions of sleep spindles and slow-wave sleep to learning outcomes.

Methods: Fifty-three adolescents were randomly assigned to an 8 h Nap schedule (6.5 h nocturnal sleep with a 90-minute daytime nap) or an 8 h No-Nap, nocturnal-only sleep schedule. The study spanned 14 nights, simulating successive school weeks. We utilized a transitive inference task involving hierarchically ordered faces. Initial learning to set up the schema was followed by rearrangement of the hierarchy (accommodation) and hierarchy expansion (assimilation). The expanded sequence was restudied. Recall of hierarchical knowledge was tested after initial learning and at multiple points for all subsequent phases. As a control, both groups underwent a No-schema condition where the hierarchy was introduced and modified without opportunity to set up a schema. Electroencephalography accompanied the multiple sleep opportunities.

Results: There were main effects of Nap schedule and Schema condition evidenced by superior recall of initial learning, reordered and expanded memoranda. Improved recall was consistently associated with higher fast spindle density but not slow-wave measures. This was true for both nocturnal sleep and daytime naps.

Conclusion: A sleep schedule incorporating regular nap opportunities compared to one that only had nocturnal sleep benefited building of robust and flexible schemas, facilitating recall of the subsequently rearranged and expanded structured knowledge. These benefits appear to be strongly associated with fast spindles.

Clinical trial registration: NCT04044885 (https://ichgcp.net/clinical-trials-registry/NCT04044885).

Keywords: memory consolidation; memory reactivation; nap; prior knowledge; schema; sleep spindles.

© Sleep Research Society 2022. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Sleep Research Society.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Study protocol and schema-learning sessions. The experiment consisted of two baseline adaptation nights and two sleep manipulation periods which included two recovery nights. Polysomnography was conducted during nights and naps on B2, M15, R11, M21, M23, and R21. The schema-learning paradigm consisted of four phases, each involving immediate and delayed test sessions: Initial learning (immediate test, 12-h test), Schema accommodation (immediate test: 12-h test, 24-h test), Schema assimilation (immediate test, 12-h test, 24-h test, 84-h test), and Schema restudy (immediate test, 12-h test, 72-h test).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
(A) (1) Phase 1a: Learning the schema to criterion. Participants performed learning blocks with active feedback (correct answer highlighted with green). Examples of test trials are shown for (2) phase 1 adjacent pairs, and (3) phase 1 inference pairs. (B) Schema-related memory integration and introduction of the No-schema condition. Participants were shown six alternating learning (with feedback) and test (with no feedback) blocks for the Schema condition. This was repeated for the No-schema condition. L, learning; T, Testing.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
(A) Schema accommodation (learning reordered material). In each condition, the position of four individuals in each hierarchy was changed. (B) Schema assimilation (additions to reordered material). In this phase, three novel items were added to each hierarchy.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Behavioral results across all phases. Performance on immediate and delayed test sessions for the Nap and No-Nap groups are shown for both the Schema and No-schema conditions. **p < .01, *p < .05.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
Correlation between schema benefit and spindle density across multiple nap sessions. Higher schema-driven memory benefits after assimilation were associated with increased N2 fast spindle density across naps (M15, M21, M23).
Figure 6.
Figure 6.
Correlation between schema benefit and spindle density across multiple nights of sleep. Higher schema-driven memory benefits after assimilation and restudy were associated with increased N2 fast spindle density across manipulation nights (M15, R11, M21, and R21).

References

    1. Gilboa A, et al. Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017;21(8):618–631.
    1. Fernandez G, et al. Memory, novelty and prior knowledge. Trends Neurosci. 2018;41(10):654–659.
    1. Bartlett FC. Remembering: An Experimental and Social Study. Cambridge: Cambridge University; 1932.
    1. Ghosh VE, et al. What is a memory schema? A historical perspective on current neuroscience literature. Neuropsychologia. 2014;53:104–114.
    1. McClelland JL. Incorporating rapid neocortical learning of new schema-consistent information into complementary learning systems theory. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2013;142(4):1190–1210.
    1. van Kesteren MTR, et al. Congruency and reactivation aid memory integration through reinstatement of prior knowledge. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):4776.
    1. Lewis PA, et al. Overlapping memory replay during sleep builds cognitive schemata. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(8):343–351.
    1. van Kesteren MTR, et al. How to optimize knowledge construction in the brain. npj Sci Learn. 2020;5:5.
    1. Dudai Y, et al. The consolidation and transformation of memory. Neuron. 2015;88(1):20–32.
    1. Wagner U, et al. Sleep inspires insight. Nature. 2004;427(6972):352–355.
    1. Monaghan P, et al. Sleep promotes analogical transfer in problem solving. Cognition. 2015;143:25–30.
    1. Ellenbogen JM, et al. Human relational memory requires time and sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104(18):7723–7728.
    1. Werchan DM, et al. Generalizing memories over time: sleep and reinforcement facilitate transitive inference. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013;100:70–76.
    1. Lo JC, et al. Cognitive effects of multi-night adolescent sleep restriction: current data and future possibilities. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2020;33:34–41.
    1. Cousins JN, et al. Splitting sleep between the night and a daytime nap reduces homeostatic sleep pressure and enhances long-term memory. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):5275.
    1. Richter FR, et al. Flexible updating of dynamic knowledge structures. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):2272.
    1. Antony JW, et al. Sleep spindles and memory reprocessing. Trends Neurosci. 2019;42(1):1–3.
    1. Fernandez LMJ, et al. Sleep spindles: mechanisms and functions. Physiol Rev. 2020;100(2):805–868.
    1. Antony JW, et al. Sleep spindle refractoriness segregates periods of memory reactivation. Curr Biol. 2018;28(11):1736–1743.e4.
    1. Batterink LJ, et al. Sleep-based memory processing facilitates grammatical generalization: evidence from targeted memory reactivation. Brain Lang. 2017;167:83–93.
    1. Tamminen J, et al. Sleep spindle activity is associated with the integration of new memories and existing knowledge. J Neurosci. 2010;30(43):14356–14360.
    1. Hennies N, et al. Sleep spindle density predicts the effect of prior knowledge on memory consolidation. J Neurosci. 2016;36(13):3799–3810.
    1. Lo JC, et al. Cognitive effects of split and continuous sleep schedules in adolescents differ according to total sleep opportunity. Sleep. 2020;43(12). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsaa129.
    1. Kumaran D. Schema-driven facilitation of new hierarchy learning in the transitive inference paradigm. Learn Mem. 2013;20(7):388–394.
    1. Preston AR, et al. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Curr Biol. 2013;23(17):R764–R773.
    1. Kumaran D, et al. Computations underlying social hierarchy learning: distinct neural mechanisms for updating and representing self-relevant information. Neuron. 2016;92(5):1135–1147.
    1. Aghayan Golkashani H, et al. Schema-driven memory benefits boost transitive inference in older adults. Psychol Aging. 2021;36:463–474.
    1. Jegou A, et al. Cortical reactivations during sleep spindles following declarative learning. Neuroimage. 2019;195:104–112.
    1. Patanaik A, et al. An end-to-end framework for real-time automatic sleep stage classification. Sleep. 2018;41(5). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsy041.
    1. Ibert C, et al. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specification. Westchester: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2007.
    1. Molle M, et al. Fast and slow spindles during the sleep slow oscillation: disparate coalescence and engagement in memory processing. Sleep. 2011;34(10):1411–1421. doi:10.5665/SLEEP.1290.
    1. Schabus M, et al. Hemodynamic cerebral correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104(32):13164–13169.
    1. Astill RG, et al. Sleep spindle and slow wave frequency reflect motor skill performance in primary school-age children. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:910.
    1. Bodizs R, et al. Sleep spindling and fluid intelligence across adolescent development: sex matters. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:952.
    1. Leong RLF, et al. Memory performance following napping in habitual and non-habitual nappers. Sleep. 2021;44(6). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsaa277.
    1. Goldstone A, et al. Sleep spindle characteristics in adolescents. Clin Neurophysiol. 2019;130(6):893–902.
    1. R C, et al. Analyzing human sleep EEG: a methodological primer with code implementation. Sleep Med Rev. 2020;54:101353.
    1. Banks S, et al. Neurobehavioral dynamics following chronic sleep restriction: dose-response effects of one night for recovery. Sleep. 2010;33(8):1013–1026. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.8.1013.
    1. Benjamini Y, et al. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol. 1995;57(1):289–300.
    1. van Kesteren MT, et al. Consolidation differentially modulates schema effects on memory for items and associations. PLoS One. 2013;8(2):e56155.
    1. Jenkins JG, et al. Obliviscence during sleep and waking. Am J Psychol. 1924;35(4):605–612.
    1. Tamminen J, et al. The role of sleep spindles and slow-wave activity in integrating new information in semantic memory. J Neurosci. 2013;33(39):15376–15381.
    1. Groch S, et al. Prior knowledge is essential for the beneficial effect of targeted memory reactivation during sleep. Sci Rep. 2017;7:39763.
    1. Latchoumane CV, et al. Thalamic spindles promote memory formation during sleep through triple phase-locking of cortical, thalamic, and hippocampal rhythms. Neuron. 2017;95(2):424–435.e6.
    1. Zhang J, et al. The effect of zolpidem on memory consolidation over a night of sleep. Sleep. 2020;43(11). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsaa084.
    1. Mednick SC, et al. The critical role of sleep spindles in hippocampal-dependent memory: a pharmacology study. J Neurosci. 2013;33(10):4494–4504.
    1. Gais S, et al. Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. J Neurosci. 2002;22(15):6830–6834.
    1. van Kesteren MTR, et al. Integrating educational knowledge: reactivation of prior knowledge during educational learning enhances memory integration. npj Sci Learn. 2018;3:11.
    1. Himmer L, et al. Rehearsal initiates systems memory consolidation, sleep makes it last. Sci Adv. 2019;5(4):eaav1695.
    1. Ohki T, et al. Neural mechanisms of mental schema: a triplet of delta, low beta/spindle and ripple oscillations. Eur J Neurosci. 2018;48(7):2416–2430.
    1. Pereira SIR, et al. The differing roles of NREM and REM sleep in the slow enhancement of skills and schemas. Curr Opin Physiol. 2020;15:82–88.
    1. Chatburn A, et al. Consolidation and generalisation across sleep depend on individual EEG factors and sleep spindle density. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2021;179:107384.
    1. Cowan E, et al. Sleep spindles promote the restructuring of memory representations in ventromedial prefrontal cortex through enhanced hippocampal-cortical functional connectivity. J Neurosci. 2020;40(9):1909–1919.
    1. Ackermann S, et al. No associations between interindividual differences in sleep parameters and episodic memory consolidation. Sleep. 2015;38(6):951–959. doi:10.5665/sleep.4748.
    1. Cordi MJ, et al. How robust are sleep-mediated memory benefits? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2020;67:1–7.
    1. Cordi MJ, Rasch B. No evidence for intra-individual correlations between sleep-mediated declarative memory consolidation and slow-wave sleep. Sleep. 2021;44(8). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsab034.
    1. Voderholzer U, et al. Sleep restriction over several days does not affect long-term recall of declarative and procedural memories in adolescents. Sleep Med. 2011;12(2):170–178.
    1. Cousins JN, et al. The impact of sleep deprivation on declarative memory. Prog Brain Res. 2019;246:27–53.
    1. Peiffer A, et al. The power of children’s sleep-Improved declarative memory consolidation in children compared with adults. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):9979. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-66880-3.
    1. Jones BJ, et al. Role of napping for learning across the lifespan. Curr Sleep Med Rep. 2020;6(4):290–297.
    1. R C, et al. Individual differences in frequency and topography of slow and fast sleep spindles. Front Hum Neurosci. 2017;11:433.
    1. Jobert M, et al. Topographical analysis of sleep spindle activity. Neuropsychobiology. 1992;26(4):210–217.
    1. Zeitlhofer J, et al. Topographic distribution of sleep spindles in young healthy subjects. J Sleep Res. 1997;6(3):149–155.
    1. Goldschmied JR, et al. Spindles are highly heritable as identified by different spindle detectors. Sleep. 2021;44(4). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsaa230.
    1. Reynolds CM, et al. Reliability of sleep spindle measurements in adolescents: how many nights are necessary? J Sleep Res. 2019;28(1):e12698.
    1. Tucker MA, et al. Comparing the effects of sleep and rest on memory consolidation. Nat Sci Sleep. 2020;12:79–91.
    1. Paller KA, et al. Memory and sleep: how sleep cognition can change the waking mind for the better. Annu Rev Psychol. 2021;72:123–150.
    1. Findlay G, et al. The evolving view of replay and its functions in wake and sleep. Sleep Adv. 2020;1(1):zpab002.

Source: PubMed

3
Prenumerera