Staying vigilant during recurrent sleep restriction: dose-response effects of time-in-bed and benefits of daytime napping

June Chi-Yan Lo, Tiffany B Koa, Ju Lynn Ong, Joshua J Gooley, Michael W L Chee, June Chi-Yan Lo, Tiffany B Koa, Ju Lynn Ong, Joshua J Gooley, Michael W L Chee

Abstract

Study objectives: We characterized vigilance deterioration with increasing time-on-task (ToT) during recurrent sleep restriction of different extents on simulated weekdays and recovery sleep on weekends, and tested the effectiveness of afternoon napping in ameliorating ToT-related deficits.

Methods: In the Need for Sleep studies, 194 adolescents (age = 15-19 years) underwent two baseline nights of 9-h time-in-bed (TIB), followed by two cycles of weekday manipulation nights and weekend recovery nights (9-h TIB). They were allocated 9 h, 8 h, 6.5 h, or 5 h of TIB for nocturnal sleep on weekdays. Three additional groups with 5 h or 6.5 h TIB were given an afternoon nap opportunity (5 h + 1 h, 5 h + 1.5 h, and 6.5 h + 1.5 h). ToT effects were quantified by performance change from the first 2 min to the last 2 min in a 10-min Psychomotor Vigilance Task administered daily.

Results: The 9 h and the 8 h groups showed comparable ToT effects that remained at baseline levels throughout the protocol. ToT-related deficits were greater among the 5 h and the 6.5 h groups, increased prominently in the second week of sleep restriction despite partial recuperation during the intervening recovery period and diverged between these two groups from the fifth sleep-restricted night. Daytime napping attenuated ToT effects when nocturnal sleep restriction was severe (i.e. 5-h TIB/night), and held steady at baseline levels for a milder dose of nocturnal sleep restriction when total TIB across 24 h was within the age-specific recommended sleep duration (i.e. 6.5 h + 1.5 h).

Conclusions: Reducing TIB beyond the recommended duration significantly increases ToT-associated vigilance impairment, particularly during recurrent periods of sleep restriction. Daytime napping is effective in ameliorating such decrement.

Clinical trial registration: NCT02838095, NCT03333512, and NCT04044885.

Keywords: napping; recovery sleep; sleep restriction; sustained attention; time on task; vigilance.

© Sleep Research Society 2022. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Sleep Research Society.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Experimental protocols. The study protocol lasted for 14 (9 h group) to 15 days (remaining groups). (A) The 9 h group maintained the same 9-h TIB (23:00–08:00; black bars) throughout the protocol (*day R22 is not applicable to the 9 h group because of its shorter protocol duration). All other groups started with two adaptation and baseline nights (B1 and B2) of 9-h TIB per night (black bars). This was followed by the first cycle of sleep manipulation for five nights (M11 to M15) and recovery sleep for two nights (R11 and R12; TIB = 9 h; black bars). The second cycle consisted of just three nights of sleep manipulation (M21 to M23) and two nights of recovery sleep (R21 and R22; black bars). During both sleep manipulation periods, participants had nocturnal TIBs of 5 h (5 h group; red bars), 6.5 h (6.5 h group; orange bars), or 8 h (8 h group; blue bars). Additionally, nap groups were given a nap opportunity starting at 14:00 following each sleep manipulation night. (B) Two nap groups had 5 h TIBs on the manipulation nights and were allowed to nap for either 1 h (5 h + 1 h group; shaded dark red bars) or 1.5 h (5 h + 1.5 h; dark red bars). (C) Another nap group had a 6.5-h TIB at night and 1.5-h nap opportunity (6.5 h + 1.5 h; brown bars) each day during the manipulation periods. A cognitive test battery (green bars) was administered daily at 10:00, 15:00–16:15, and 20:00, except during the first and last days of the protocols.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Time-on-task effects on the number of lapses in the Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) as a function of nocturnal TIB and daytime napping. Left panel: The means and standard errors of the number of PVT lapses in each 2-min bin, averaged across the three tests each day, are plotted from the second baseline day (B2) to the day after the first recovery night in the second cycle (R21). Right panel: The changes from the first to the fifth bin (time-on-task effect) in the number of PVT lapses, averaged across the three tests each day, are shown. The least square means and standard errors estimated with general linear mixed models are illustrated. Shaded gray areas indicate the sleep manipulation periods (M11 to M15 and M21 to M23). (A) Data are shown for the 9 h group (black), 8 h group (blue), 6.5 h group (orange), and 5 h group (red). (B) Data are plotted for the 5 h group (red), the 5 h + 1 h group (dark red open circles with dotted line), and the 5 h + 1.5 h (dark red filled circles with solid line). ^^^p < 0.001, ^^p < 0.01, and ^p < 0.05 for significant contrasts between the 5 h group and the 5 h + 1 h group. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, and *p < 0.05 for significant contrasts between the 5 h group and the 5 h + 1.5 h group. (C) Data are plotted for the 6.5 h group (orange) and the 6.5 h + 1.5 h (brown). ***p < .001, **p < .01, and *p < .05 for significant contrasts between the 6.5 h group and the 6.5 h + 1.5 h group.

References

    1. Lowe CJ, et al. The neurocognitive consequences of sleep restriction: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;80:586‐ 604.
    1. Agostini A, et al. An experimental study of adolescent sleep restriction during a simulated school week: changes in phase, sleep staging, performance and sleepiness. J Sleep Res. 2017;26(2):227‐ 235.
    1. Basner M, et al. Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (pvt) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011;34(5):581‐ 591. doi:10.1093/sleep/34.5.581.
    1. Belenky G, et al. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J Sleep Res. 2003;12(1):1‐ 12.
    1. Lo JC, et al. Effects of partial and acute total sleep deprivation on performance across cognitive domains, individuals and circadian phase. PLoS One. 2012;7(9):e45987.
    1. Lo JC, et al. Cognitive performance, sleepiness, and mood in partially sleep deprived adolescents: The Need for Sleep Study. Sleep. 2016;39(3):687‐ 698. doi:10.5665/sleep.5552.
    1. Short MA, et al. Estimating adolescent sleep need using dose-response modeling. Sleep. 2018;41(4). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsy011.
    1. Van Dongen HP, et al. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003; 26(2):117‐ 126. doi:10.1093/sleep/26.2.117.
    1. Van Dongen HPA, et al. Investigating the temporal dynamics and underlying mechanisms of cognitive fatigue. In: Ackerman PL, ed. Cognitive Fatigue: Multidisciplinary Perspectives on Current Research and Future Applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2011:124‐ 147.
    1. Hudson AN, et al. Sleep deprivation, vigilant attention, and brain function: a review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(1):21‐ 30.
    1. Hirshkowitz M, et al. National Sleep Foundation’s sleep time duration recommendations: methodology and results summary. Sleep Health. 2015;1(1):40‐ 43.
    1. Lo JC, et al. Young adults’ sleep duration on work days: differences between East and West. Front Neurol. 2014;5:81.
    1. Ong JL, et al. Large-scale data from wearables reveal regional disparities in sleep patterns that persist across age and sex. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):3415.
    1. Yeo SC, et al. Associations of sleep duration on school nights with self-rated health, overweight, and depression symptoms in adolescents: problems and possible solutions. Sleep Med. 2019;60:96‐ 108.
    1. Lo JC, et al. Neurobehavioral impact of successive cycles of sleep restriction with and without naps in adolescents. Sleep. 2017;40(2). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsw042.
    1. Lo JC, et al. Differential effects of split and continuous sleep on neurobehavioral function and glucose tolerance in sleep-restricted adolescents. Sleep. 2019;42(5). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsz037.
    1. Lo JC, et al. Cognitive effects of split and continuous sleep schedules in adolescents differ according to total sleep opportunity. Sleep. 2020;43(12). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsaa129.
    1. Lim J, et al. Assessing the benefits of napping and short rest breaks on processing speed in sleep-restricted adolescents. J Sleep Res. 2017;26(2):219‐ 226.
    1. Buysse DJ, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193‐ 213.
    1. Horne JA, et al. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4(2):97‐ 110.
    1. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1991;14(6):540‐ 545. doi:10.1093/sleep/14.6.540.
    1. Meijer AM. Chronic sleep reduction, functioning at school and school achievement in preadolescents. J Sleep Res. 2008;17(4):395‐ 405.
    1. Rogers NL, et al. Interaction of chronic sleep restriction and circadian system in humans. J Sleep Res. 2008;17(4):406‐ 411.
    1. Ong JL, et al. EEG changes across multiple nights of sleep restriction and recovery in adolescents: the Need for Sleep study. Sleep. 2016;39(6):1233‐ 1240. doi:10.5665/sleep.5840.
    1. Ong JL, et al. EEG changes accompanying successive cycles of sleep restriction with and without naps in adolescents. Sleep. 2017;40(4). doi:10.1093/sleep/zsx030.
    1. Dinges DF, et al. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1985;17(6):652‐ 655.
    1. Grant DA, et al. 3-Minute smartphone-based and tablet-based psychomotor vigilance tests for the assessment of reduced alertness due to sleep deprivation. Behav Res Methods. 2017;49(3):1020‐ 1029.
    1. Hansen DA, et al. Psychomotor vigilance impairment during total sleep deprivation is exacerbated in sleep-onset insomnia. Nat Sci Sleep. 2019;11:401‐ 410.
    1. Jung CM, et al. Comparison of sustained attention assessed by auditory and visual psychomotor vigilance tasks prior to and during sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2011;20(2):348‐ 355.
    1. Tucker AM, et al. The variable response-stimulus interval effect and sleep deprivation: an unexplored aspect of psychomotor vigilance task performance. Sleep. 2009;32(10):1393‐ 1395. doi:10.1093/sleep/32.10.1393.
    1. Paruthi S, et al. . Recommended amount of sleep for pediatric populations: a consensus statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. J Clin Sleep Med. 2016;12(6):785‐ 786.
    1. Banks S, et al. . Neurobehavioral dynamics following chronic sleep restriction: dose-response effects of one night for recovery. Sleep. 2010;33(8):1013‐ 1026. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.8.1013.
    1. Lim J, et al. Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: an ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect. Neuroimage. 2010;49(4):3426‐ 3435.
    1. Asplund CL, et al. Time-on-task and sleep deprivation effects are evidenced in overlapping brain areas. Neuroimage. 2013;82:326‐ 335.
    1. Massar SAA, et al. . Trait-like nocturnal sleep behavior identified by combining wearable, phone-use, and self-report data. NPJ Digit Med. 2021;4(1):90.
    1. Adam M, et al. Age-related changes in the time course of vigilant attention during 40 hours without sleep in men. Sleep. 2006;29(1):55‐ 57. doi:10.1093/sleep/29.1.55.

Source: PubMed

3
Prenumerera