Randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover study of single-dose guanfacine in unilateral neglect following stroke

Edwin S Dalmaijer, Korina M S Li, Nikos Gorgoraptis, Alexander P Leff, David L Cohen, Andrew D Parton, Masud Husain, Paresh A Malhotra, Edwin S Dalmaijer, Korina M S Li, Nikos Gorgoraptis, Alexander P Leff, David L Cohen, Andrew D Parton, Masud Husain, Paresh A Malhotra

Abstract

Objective: Unilateral neglect is a poststroke disorder that impacts negatively on functional outcome and lacks established, effective treatment. This multicomponent syndrome is characterised by a directional bias of attention away from contralesional space, together with impairments in several cognitive domains, including sustained attention and spatial working memory. This study aimed to test the effects of guanfacine, a noradrenergic alpha-2A agonist, on ameliorating aspects of neglect.

Methods: Thirteen right hemisphere stroke patients with leftward neglect were included in a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled proof-of-concept crossover study that examined the effects of a single dose of guanfacine. Patients were tested on a computerised, time-limited cancellation paradigm, as well as tasks that independently assessed sustained attention and spatial working memory.

Results: On guanfacine, there was a statistically significant improvement in the total number of targets found on the cancellation task when compared with placebo (mean improvement of 5, out of a possible 64). However, there was no evidence of a change in neglect patients' directional attention bias. Furthermore, Bayesian statistical analysis revealed reliable evidence against any effects of guanfacine on search organisation and performance on our sustained attention and spatial working memory tasks.

Conclusions: Guanfacine improves search in neglect by boosting the number of targets found but had no effects on directional bias or search organisation, nor did it improve sustained attention or working memory on independent tasks. Further work is necessary to determine whether longer term treatment with guanfacine may be effective for some neglect patients and whether it affects functional outcome measures.

Trial registration number: NCT00955253.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing interests: None declared.

© Article author(s) (or their employer(s) unless otherwise stated in the text of the article) 2018. All rights reserved. No commercial use is permitted unless otherwise expressly granted.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Individual lesion maps for all 13 participants. Patients 1001–1010 had some degree of cortical frontal involvement, whereas patients 2001–2003 did not.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Total number of correctly marked targets in a cancellation task (A), as well as the difference between the number of correctly marked targets on the right and the left sides of the task (B) and the centre of cancellation where positive values indicate a rightward bias (C) in the baseline (green) condition and after placebo (blue) or guanfacine (red) administration. Solid horizontal lines indicate the mean, and error bars indicate within-participant 95% CIs. Each set of three connected dots represents a participant (grey for patients with cortical frontal involvement, and orange for without).

References

    1. Ringman JM, Saver JL, Woolson RF, et al. . Frequency, risk factors, anatomy, and course of unilateral neglect in an acute stroke cohort. Neurology 2004;63:468–74. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000133011.10689.CE
    1. Bartolomeo P, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Chica AB, Schotten Tde. Brain networks of visuospatial attention and their disruption in visual neglect. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00110
    1. Kortte K, Hillis AE. Recent advances in the understanding of neglect and anosognosia following right hemisphere stroke. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2009;9:459–65. 10.1007/s11910-009-0068-8
    1. Nys GM, van Zandvoort MJ, de Kort PL, et al. . The prognostic value of domain-specific cognitive abilities in acute first-ever stroke. Neurology 2005;64:821–7. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152984.28420.5A
    1. Azouvi P, Jacquin-Courtois S, Luauté J. Rehabilitation of unilateral neglect: Evidence-based medicine. Ann Phys Rehabil Med 2017;60:191–7. 10.1016/j.rehab.2016.10.006
    1. Luvizutto GJ, Bazan R, Braga GP, et al. . Pharmacological interventions for unilateral spatial neglect after stroke : Cochrane Stroke Group, Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Vol 12 New Jersey, USA: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, 2015. 10.1002/14651858.CD010882.pub2
    1. van der Kemp J, Dorresteijn M, Ten Brink AF, et al. . Pharmacological treatment of visuospatial neglect: a systematic review. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis 2017;26:686–700. 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2017.02.012
    1. Husain M, Rorden C. Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003;4:26–36. 10.1038/nrn1005
    1. Robertson IH, Manly T, Beschin N, et al. . Auditory sustained attention is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 1997;35:1527–32. 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00084-5
    1. Robertson IH, Mattingley JB, Rorden C, et al. . Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness. Nature 1998;395:169–72. 10.1038/25993
    1. George MS, Mercer JS, Walker R, et al. . A demonstration of endogenous modulation of unilateral spatial neglect: the impact of apparent time-pressure on spatial bias. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14:33-41 10.1017/S135561770808003X
    1. Aston-Jones G, Rajkowski J, Kubiak P, Alexinsky T, et al. . Locus coeruleus neurons in monkey are selectively activated by attended cues in a vigilance task. J Neurosci 1994;14:4467–80.
    1. Smith A, Nutt D. Noradrenaline and attention lapses. Nature 1996;380:291 10.1038/380291a0
    1. Malhotra PA, Parton AD, Greenwood R, et al. . Noradrenergic modulation of space exploration in visual neglect. Ann Neurol 2006;59:186–90. 10.1002/ana.20701
    1. Biederman J, Melmed RD, Patel A, et al. . A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of guanfacine extended release in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 2008;121:e73–e84. 10.1542/peds.2006-3695
    1. Jäkälä P, Riekkinen M, Sirviö J, et al. . Guanfacine, but not clonidine, improves planning and working memory performance in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999;20:460–70. 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00127-4
    1. Arnsten AF, Cai JX, Goldman-Rakic PS. The alpha-2 adrenergic agonist guanfacine improves memory in aged monkeys without sedative or hypotensive side effects: evidence for alpha-2 receptor subtypes. J Neurosci 1988;8:4287–98.
    1. Arnsten AF, Contant TA. Alpha-2 adrenergic agonists decrease distractibility in aged monkeys performing the delayed response task. Psychopharmacology 1992;108:159–69. 10.1007/BF02245302
    1. Avery RA, Franowicz JS, Studholme C, et al. . The alpha-2A-adrenoceptor agonist, guanfacine, increases regional cerebral blood flow in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of monkeys performing a spatial working memory task. Neuropsychopharmacology 2000;23:240–9. 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00111-1
    1. Parton A, Malhotra P, Nachev P, et al. . Space re-exploration in hemispatial neglect. Neuroreport 2006;17:833–6. 10.1097/01.wnr.0000220130.86349.a7
    1. Ji XH, Ji JZ, Zhang H, et al. . Stimulation of alpha2-adrenoceptors suppresses excitatory synaptic transmission in the medial prefrontal cortex of rat. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008;33:2263–71. 10.1038/sj.npp.1301603
    1. Singh-Curry V, Malhotra P, Farmer SF, et al. . Attention deficits following ADEM ameliorated by guanfacine. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;82:688–90. 10.1136/jnnp.2009.195792
    1. Gorgoraptis N, Mah YH, Machner B, et al. . The effects of the dopamine agonist rotigotine on hemispatial neglect following stroke. Brain 2012;135:2478–91. 10.1093/brain/aws154
    1. Mesulam M-M. Principle of Behavioural Neurology: tests of directed attention and memory. USA: F.A. Davis Company, 1985.
    1. Wilson BA, Cockburn J, Halligan P. Behavioural inattention test: manual. Suffolk: Thames Valley Test Company, 1987.
    1. Riccio CA, Reynolds CR, Lowe PA. Clinical applications of continuous performance tests. New Jersey, USA: John Wiley & Sons, 2001.
    1. Wojciulik E, Rorden C, Clarke K, et al. . Group study of an ‘undercover’ test for visuospatial neglect: invisible cancellation can reveal more neglect than standard cancellation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004;75:1356–8. 10.1136/jnnp.2003.021931
    1. Dalmaijer ES, Van der Stigchel S, Nijboer TCW, et al. . CancellationTools: All-in-one software for administration and analysis of cancellation tasks. Behav Res Methods 2015;47:1065–75. 10.3758/s13428-014-0522-7
    1. Binder J, Marshall R, Lazar R, et al. . Distinct syndromes of hemineglect. Arch Neurol 1992;49:1187–94. 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530350109026
    1. Stanislaw H, Todorov N. Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 1999;31:137–49. 10.3758/BF03207704
    1. Malhotra P, Jäger HR, Parton A, et al. . Spatial working memory capacity in unilateral neglect. Brain 2005;128:424–35. 10.1093/brain/awh372
    1. Wagenmakers EJ. A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychon Bull Rev 2007;14:779–804. 10.3758/BF03194105
    1. Masson ME. A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behav Res Methods 2011;43:679–90. 10.3758/s13428-010-0049-5
    1. Jeffreys H. Theory of probability. UK: Clarendon Press, 1961.
    1. Dalmaijer ES. Python for experimental psychologists. UK: Routledge, 2017.
    1. Van Rossum G, Drake FL. Python Language reference manual: Network Theory Ltd, 2011.
    1. Oliphant TE. Python for Scientific Computing. Comput Sci Eng 2007;9:10–20. 10.1109/MCSE.2007.58
    1. Hunter JD. Matplotlib: A 2D Graphics Environment. Comput Sci Eng 2007;9:90–5. 10.1109/MCSE.2007.55
    1. JASP Team. JASP. 2016.
    1. Rabuffetti M, Farina E, Alberoni M, et al. . Spatio-temporal features of visual exploration in unilaterally brain-damaged subjects with or without neglect: results from a touchscreen test. PLoS One 2012;7:e31511 10.1371/journal.pone.0031511
    1. Ten Brink AF, Van der Stigchel S, Visser-Meily JM, et al. . You never know where you are going until you know where you have been: Disorganized search after stroke. J Neuropsychol 2016;10 10.1111/jnp.12068
    1. Ten Brink AF, Biesbroek JM, Kuijf HJ, et al. . The right hemisphere is dominant in organization of visual search-A study in stroke patients. Behav Brain Res 2016;304:71–9. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.02.004
    1. Jehkonen M, Ahonen JP, Dastidar P, et al. . Visual neglect as a predictor of functional outcome one year after stroke. Acta Neurol Scand 2000;101:195–201. 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2000.101003195.x

Source: PubMed

3
Prenumerera