Breastfeeding during infancy and neurocognitive function in adolescence: 16-year follow-up of the PROBIT cluster-randomized trial

Seungmi Yang, Richard M Martin, Emily Oken, Mikhail Hameza, Glen Doniger, Shimon Amit, Rita Patel, Jennifer Thompson, Sheryl L Rifas-Shiman, Konstantin Vilchuck, Natalia Bogdanovich, Michael S Kramer, Seungmi Yang, Richard M Martin, Emily Oken, Mikhail Hameza, Glen Doniger, Shimon Amit, Rita Patel, Jennifer Thompson, Sheryl L Rifas-Shiman, Konstantin Vilchuck, Natalia Bogdanovich, Michael S Kramer

Abstract

Background: Evidence on the long-term effect of breastfeeding on neurocognitive development is based almost exclusively on observational studies. In the 16-year follow-up study of a large, cluster-randomized trial of a breastfeeding promotion intervention, we evaluated the long-term persistence of the neurocognitive benefits of the breastfeeding promotion intervention previously observed at early school age.

Methods and findings: A total of 13,557 participants (79.5% of the 17,046 randomized) of the Promotion of Breastfeeding Intervention Trial (PROBIT) were followed up at age 16 from September 2012 to July 2015. At the follow-up, neurocognitive function was assessed in 7 verbal and nonverbal cognitive domains using a computerized, self-administered test battery among 13,427 participants. Using an intention-to-treat (ITT) analysis as our prespecified primary analysis, we estimated cluster- and baseline characteristic-adjusted mean differences between the intervention (prolonged and exclusive breastfeeding promotion modelled on the Baby-Friendly Hospital Initiative) and control (usual care) groups in 7 cognitive domains and a global cognitive score. In our prespecified secondary analysis, we estimated mean differences by instrumental variable (IV) analysis to account for noncompliance with the randomly assigned intervention and estimate causal effects of breastfeeding. The 16-year follow-up rates were similar in the intervention (79.7%) and control groups (79.3%), and baseline characteristics were comparable between the two. In the cluster-adjusted ITT analyses, children in the intervention group did not show statistically significant differences in the scores from children in the control group. Prespecified additional adjustment for baseline characteristics improved statistical precision and resulted in slightly higher scores among children in the intervention for verbal function (1.4 [95% CI 0.3-2.5]) and memory (1.2 [95% CI 0.01-2.4]). IV analysis showed that children who were exclusively breastfed for ≥3 (versus <3) months had a 3.5-point (95% CI 0.9-6.1) higher verbal function, but no differences were observed in other domains. While our computerized, self-administered cognitive testing reduced the cluster-level variability in the scores, it may have increased individual-level measurement errors in adolescents.

Conclusions: We observed no benefit of a breastfeeding promotion intervention on overall neurocognitive function. The only beneficial effect was on verbal function at age 16. The higher verbal ability is consistent with results observed at early school age; however, the effect size was substantially smaller in adolescence.

Probit trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT01561612.

Conflict of interest statement

I have read the journal's policy and the authors of this manuscript have the following competing interests: GD and SA are employees of NeuroTrax.

Figures

Fig 1. Flow diagram of clusters and…
Fig 1. Flow diagram of clusters and participants of PROBIT recruitment follow-up phases at 12 mo, 6.5 y, 11.5 y, and 16 y.
a: During the 11.5-y follow-up, 6 deaths were reported in the intervention arm. Data-checking during the 16-y follow-up found one of these children had been incorrectly reported as deceased and data were amended. b: Of the 13,557 seen at the 16-y follow-up, 12,072 were seen at both 11.5-y and 16-y follow-ups, 274 were not seen at either 6.5-y or 11.5-y follow-ups, 449 were seen at 6.5 y but not seen at 11.5 y, and 762 were seen at 11.5 y but not seen at 6.5 y. Of the 3,489 children randomized but not followed up at 16 y, 267 attended the excluded site, 116 died after randomization, 2,674 were lost to follow-up, and 432 were unable or unwilling to come for their clinic visit.

References

    1. Horta BL, Victora CG. Long-term effects of breastfeeding: a systematic review Geneva: World Health Organization, 2013.
    1. Horta BL, Loret de Mola C, Victora CG. Breastfeeding and intelligence: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104:14–9. doi:
    1. Victora CG, Horta BL, Loret de Mola C, Quevedo L, Pinheiro RT, Gigante DP, et al. Association between breastfeeding and intelligence, educational attainment, and income at 30 years of age: a prospective birth cohort study from Brazil. Lancet Glob Health. 2015;3(4):e199–205. doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4365917.
    1. Brion M-JA, Lawlor DA, Matijasevich A, Horta B, Anselmi L, Araújo CL, et al. What are the causal effects of breastfeeding on IQ, obesity and blood pressure? Evidence from comparing high-income with middle-income cohorts. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40(3):670–80. doi:
    1. Der G, Batty GD, Deary IJ. Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children: prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2006;333(7575):945 doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1633819.
    1. Evenhouse E, Reilly S. Improved estimates of the benefits of breastfeeding using sibling comparisons to reduce selection bias. Health Serv Res. 2005;40(6 Pt 1):1781–802. doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1361236.
    1. Gillman MW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Berkey CS, Frazier AL, Rockett HR, Camargo CA Jr., et al. Breast-feeding and overweight in adolescence: within-family analysis [corrected]. Epidemiology. 2006;17(1):112–4. doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1994917.
    1. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, Gore SM, Lucas PJ, Crowle P, et al. Early diet in preterm babies and developmental status at 18 months. Lancet. 1990;335(8704):1477–81. Epub 1990/06/23. .
    1. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, Lister G, Leeson-Payne C. Breast milk and subsequent intelligence quotient in children born preterm. Lancet. 1992;339(8788):261–4. .
    1. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, Gore SM. A randomised multicentre study of human milk versus formula and later development in preterm infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1994;70(2):F141–6. ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1061016.
    1. Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E, Vanilovich I, Platt RW, Matush L, et al. Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized trial. JAMA Psychiatry. 2008;65(5):578–84. doi: .
    1. Kramer MS, Martin RM, Sterne JAC, Shapiro S, Dahhou M, Platt RW. The "double jeopardy" of clustered measurement and cluster randomization. BMJ. 2009;339(7719):501–3.
    1. Kramer MS, Chalmers B, Hodnett ED, Sevkovskaya Z, Dzikovich I, Shapiro S, et al. Promotion of Breastfeeding Intervention Trial (PROBIT): a randomized trial in the Republic of Belarus. JAMA. 2001;285(4):413–20. .
    1. Guthrie LB, Oken E, Sterne JA, Gillman MW, Patel R, Vilchuck K, et al. Ongoing monitoring of data clustering in multicenter studies. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2012;12:29 doi: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3313871.
    1. Doniger GM, Okun MS, Simon ES, Rodriguez RL, Jacobson CE, Weiss D, et al. Validation of a computerized neuropsychological assessment (Mindstreams) in movement disorders: Interim analysis. Mov Disord. 2006;21(9):1557–. PubMed PMID: WOS:000240998100096.
    1. Schweiger A, Abramovitch A, Doniger GM, Simon ES. A clinical construct validity study of a novel computerized battery for the diagnosis of ADHD in young adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29(1):100–11. doi: PubMed PMID: WOS:000242767700010.
    1. Paleacu D, Shutzman A, Giladi N, Herman T, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Effects of pharmacological therapy on gait and cognitive function in depressed patients. Clin Neuropharmacol. 2007;30(2):63–71. doi: PubMed PMID: WOS:000245529000001.
    1. Ritsner MS, Blumenkrantz H, Dubinsky T, Dwolatzky T. The detection of neurocognitive decline in schizophrenia using the Mindstreams Computerized Cognitive Test Battery. Schizophr Res. 2006;82(1):39–49. doi: .
    1. White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med. 2011;30(4):377–99. doi: PubMed PMID: WOS:000287106200008.
    1. Rubin DB. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1987.
    1. Angrist JD, Imbens GW, Rubin DB. Identification of causal effects using instrumental variables. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1996;91(434):444–55. doi: PubMed PMID: WOS:A1996UP55200002.
    1. Sterne JA, Davey Smith G. Sifting the evidence-what's wrong with significance tests? BMJ. 2001;322(7280):226–31. .
    1. Wasserstein RL, Assoc AS. ASA Statement on Statistical Significance and P-Values. American Statistician. 2016;70(2):131–3. PubMed PMID: WOS:000378462300002.
    1. Wasserstein RL, Lazar NA. The ASA's Statement on p-Values: Context, Process, and Purpose. American Statistician. 2016;70(2):129–31. doi: PubMed PMID: WOS:000378462300001.
    1. Batty GD, Deary IJ. Early life intelligence and adult health. BMJ. 2004;329(7466):585–6. doi: .
    1. Fulker DW, Defries JC, Plomin R. Genetic Influence on General Mental-Ability Increases between Infancy and Middle Childhood. Nature. 1988;336(6201):767–9. doi: PubMed PMID: WOS:A1988R536500056.
    1. Nisbett RE, Aronson J, Blair C, Dickens W, Flynn J, Halpern DF, et al. Intelligence New Findings and Theoretical Developments. Am Psychol. 2012;67(2):130–59. doi: PubMed PMID: WOS:000300538200005.
    1. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ. Randomised trial of early diet in preterm babies and later intelligence quotient. BMJ. 1998;317(7171):1481–7. ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC28727.
    1. Farquharson J, Cockburn F, Patrick WA, Jamieson EC, Logan RW. Infant cerebral cortex phospholipid fatty-acid composition and diet. Lancet. 1992;340(8823):810–3. .
    1. Koletzko B, Agostoni C, Carlson SE, Clandinin T, Hornstra G, Neuringer M, et al. Long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LC-PUFA) and perinatal development. Acta Paediatr. 2001;90(4):460–4. .
    1. Jasani B, Simmer K, Patole SK, Rao SC. Long chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in infants born at term. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017;3:CD000376 doi: .
    1. Britton JR, Britton HL, Gronwaldt V. Breastfeeding, sensitivity, and attachment. Pediatrics. 2006;118(5):e1436–43. doi: .
    1. Zhou SJ, Baghurst P, Gibson RA, Makrides M. Home environment, not duration of breast-feeding, predicts intelligence quotient of children at four years. Nutrition. 2007;23(3):236–41. doi: .
    1. Boutwell BB, Beaver KM, Barnes JC. Role of breastfeeding in childhood cognitive development: A propensity score matching analysis. J Paediatr Child Health. 2012;48(9):840–5. doi:

Source: PubMed

3
Prenumerera