Effects of the Visual Exercise Environments on Cognitive Directed Attention, Energy Expenditure and Perceived Exertion

Mike Rogerson, Jo Barton, Mike Rogerson, Jo Barton

Abstract

Green exercise research often reports psychological health outcomes without rigorously controlling exercise. This study examines effects of visual exercise environments on directed attention, perceived exertion and time to exhaustion, whilst measuring and controlling the exercise component. Participants completed three experimental conditions in a randomized counterbalanced order. Conditions varied by video content viewed (nature; built; control) during two consistently-ordered exercise bouts (Exercise 1: 60% VO2peakInt for 15-mins; Exercise 2: 85% VO2peakInt to voluntary exhaustion). In each condition, participants completed modified Backwards Digit Span tests (a measure of directed attention) pre- and post-Exercise 1. Energy expenditure, respiratory exchange ratio and perceived exertion were measured during both exercise bouts. Time to exhaustion in Exercise 2 was also recorded. There was a significant time by condition interaction for Backwards Digit Span scores (F2,22 = 6.267, p = 0.007). Scores significantly improved in the nature condition (p < 0.001) but did not in the built or control conditions. There were no significant differences between conditions for either perceived exertion or physiological measures during either Exercise 1 or Exercise 2, or for time to exhaustion in Exercise 2. This was the first study to demonstrate effects of controlled exercise conducted in different visual environments on post-exercise directed attention. Via psychological mechanisms alone, visual nature facilitates attention restoration during moderate-intensity exercise.

Keywords: cognitive functioning; directed attention; exercise environments; green exercise; perceived exertion; wellbeing.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Mean (±SD) pre- and post- Exercise 1 Backwards Digit Span test scores by condition; Higher score represents greater level of directed attention; *, pre- and post- Exercise 1 values significantly differ (p = 0.001); , significant time by condition interaction (p = 0.007).
Figure 2
Figure 2
Mean (±SD) energy expenditure and respiratory exchange ratio by condition for Exercise 1 and Exercise 2; Exercise 1 duration was 15-mins, Exercise 2 values each represent 4:47-mins of data.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Perceived exertion scores by exercise bout, time and condition; Rated perceived exertion: minimum value = 6 (no exertion at all), maximum value = 20 (maximal exertion).

References

    1. Pretty J., Peacock J., Sellens M., Griffin M. The mental and physical health outcomes of green exercise. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2005;15:319–337. doi: 10.1080/09603120500155963.
    1. Brown D.K., Barton J.L., Pretty J., Gladwell V. Walks4work: Assessing the role of the natural environment in a workplace physical activity intervention. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health. 2014;40:390–399. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.3421.
    1. Lee J., Li Q., Tyrväinen L., Tsunetsugu Y., Park B.-J., Kagawa T., Miyazaki Y. Nature therapy and preventive medicine. Public Health-Soc. Behav. Health. 2012;16:325–350.
    1. Park B.J., Tsunetsugu Y., Kasetani T., Kagawa T., Miyazaki Y. The physiological effects of shinrin-yoku (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing): Evidence from field experiments in 24 forests across Japan. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 2010;15:18–26. doi: 10.1007/s12199-009-0086-9.
    1. Berman M.G., Jonides J., Kaplan S. The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychol. Sci. 2008;19:1207–1212. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02225.x.
    1. Hartig T., Evans G.W., Jamner L.D., Davis D.S., Gärling T. Tracking restoration in natural and urban field settings. J. Environ. Psychol. 2003;23:109–123. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00109-3.
    1. Focht B.C. Brief walks in outdoor and laboratory environments: Effects on affective responses, enjoyment, and intentions to walk for exercise. Res. Quart. Exerc. Sport. 2009;80:611–620. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2009.10599600.
    1. Teas J., Hurley T., Ghumare S., Ogoussan K. Walking outside improves mood for healthy postmenopausal women. Clin. Med. Oncol. 2007;1:35–43.
    1. Ryan R.M., Weinstein N., Bernstein J., Brown K.W., Mistretta L., Gagne M. Vitalizing effects of being outdoors and in nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010;30:159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.10.009.
    1. Akers A., Barton J., Cossey R., Gainsford P., Griffin M., Micklewright D. Visual color perception in green exercise: Positive effects on mood and perceived exertion. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2012;46:8661–8666. doi: 10.1021/es301685g.
    1. Wood C., Angus C., Pretty J., Sandercock G., Barton J. A randomised control trial of physical activity in a perceived environment on self-esteem and mood in UK adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2013;23:311–320. doi: 10.1080/09603123.2012.733935.
    1. Thompson Coon J., Boddy K., Stein K., Whear R., Barton J., Depledge M.H. Does participating in physical activity in outdoor natural environments have a greater effect on physical and mental wellbeing than physical activity indoors? A systematic review. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2011;45:1761–1772. doi: 10.1021/es102947t.
    1. Kerr J.H., Fujiyama H., Sugano A., Okamura T., Chang M., Onouha F. Psychological responses to exercising in laboratory and natural environments. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2006;7:345–359. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2005.09.002.
    1. Rogerson M., Brown D.K., Sandercock G., Wooller J.-J., Barton J. A comparison of four typical green exercise environments and prediction of psychological health outcomes. Perspect. Public Health. 2015 doi: 10.1177/1757913915589845.
    1. Barton J., Pretty J. What is the best dose of nature and green exercise for improving mental health? A multi-study analysis. Environ. Sci. Technol. 2010;44:3947–3955. doi: 10.1021/es903183r.
    1. Kaplan S., Berman M.G. Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2010;5:43–57. doi: 10.1177/1745691609356784.
    1. Itti L., Rees G., Tsotsos J.K. Neurobiology of Attention. Academic Press; Waltham, MA, USA: 2005.
    1. Kaplan S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 1995;15:169–182. doi: 10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2.
    1. Dietrich A. Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Conscious. Cogn. 2003;12:231–256. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8100(02)00046-6.
    1. Dietrich A., Sparling P.B. Endurance exercise selectively impairs prefrontal-dependent cognition. Brain Cogn. 2004;55:516–524. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.03.002.
    1. Dietrich A. Transient hypofrontality as a mechanism for the psychological effects of exercise. Psychiat. Res. 2006;145:79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.033.
    1. Daffner K.R., Mesulam M., Scinto L., Acar D., Calvo V., Faust R., Chabrerie A., Kennedy B., Holcomb P. The central role of the prefrontal cortex in directing attention to novel events. Brain. 2000;123:927–939. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.5.927.
    1. Gailliot M.T., Baumeister R.F. The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2007;11:303–327. doi: 10.1177/1088868307303030.
    1. Milham M., Banich M., Webb A., Barad V., Cohen N., Wszalek T., Kramer A. The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. Cogn. Brain Res. 2001;12:467–473. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00076-3.
    1. Miller E.K., Cohen J.D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167.
    1. Labelle V., Bosquet L., Mekary S., Bherer L. Decline in executive control during acute bouts of exercise as a function of exercise intensity and fitness level. Brain Cogn. 2013;81:10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.10.001.
    1. Yanagisawa H., Dan I., Tsuzuki D., Kato M., Okamoto M., Kyutoku Y., Soya H. Acute moderate exercise elicits increased dorsolateral prefrontal activation and improves cognitive performance with stroop test. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1702–1710. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.023.
    1. Byun K., Hyodo K., Suwabe K., Ochi G., Sakairi Y., Kato M., Dan I., Soya H. Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: An fnirs study. NeuroImage. 2014;98:336–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.067.
    1. Kaplan R., Kaplan S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY, USA: 1989.
    1. Kaplan S. Meditation, restoration, and the management of mental fatigue. Environ. Behav. 2001;33:480–506. doi: 10.1177/00139160121973106.
    1. Ottosson J., Grahn P. A comparison of leisure time spent in a garden with leisure time spent indoors: On measures of restoration in residents in geriatric care. Landsc. Res. 2005;30:23–55. doi: 10.1080/0142639042000324758.
    1. Berto R. Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. J. Environ. Psychol. 2005;25:249–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.07.001.
    1. Kaplan R. The role of nature in the context of the workplace. Landsc. Urban Plan. 1993;26:193–201. doi: 10.1016/0169-2046(93)90016-7.
    1. Raanaas R.K., Evensen K.H., Rich D., Sjøstrøm G., Patil G. Benefits of indoor plants on attention capacity in an office setting. J. Environ. Psychol. 2011;31:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.11.005.
    1. Pretty J. How nature contributes to mental and physical health. Spiritual. Health Int. 2004;5:68–78. doi: 10.1002/shi.220.
    1. Weinstein N., Przybylski A.K., Ryan R.M. Can nature make us more caring? Effects of immersion in nature on intrinsic aspirations and generosity. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009;35:1315–1329. doi: 10.1177/0146167209341649.
    1. Macera C.A., Hootman J.M., Sniezek J.E. Major public health benefits of physical activity. Arthritis Care Res. 2003;49:122–128. doi: 10.1002/art.10907.
    1. Thompson P.D., Lim V. Physical activity in the prevention of atherosclerotic coronary heart disease. Curr. Treat. Options Cardiovasc. Med. 2003;5:279–285. doi: 10.1007/s11936-003-0027-7.
    1. Ekkekakis P., Parfitt G., Petruzzello S.J. The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they exercise at different intensities. Sports Med. 2011;41:641–671. doi: 10.2165/11590680-000000000-00000.
    1. Kashihara K., Maruyama T., Murota M., Nakahara Y. Positive effects of acute and moderate physical exercise on cognitive function. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2009;28:155–164. doi: 10.2114/jpa2.28.155.
    1. Kilpatrick M., Kraemer R., Bartholomew J., Acevedo E., Jarreau D. Affective responses to exercise are dependent on intensity rather than total work. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2007;39 doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31806ad73c.
    1. Tomporowski P.D. Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol. 2003;112:297–324. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8.
    1. Borg G. Perceived exertion as an indicator of somatic stress. Scand. J. Rehabil. Med. 1970;2:92–98.
    1. Borg G.A. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1982;14:377–381. doi: 10.1249/00005768-198205000-00012.
    1. Borg G. Borg’s Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales. Human kinetics; Champaign, IL, USA: 1998.
    1. Ceci R., Hassmén P. Self-monitored exercise at three different rpe intensities in treadmill vs. field running. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1991;23:732–738. doi: 10.1249/00005768-199106000-00013.
    1. Ulrich R.S., Simons R.F., Losito B.D., Fiorito E., Miles M.A., Zelson M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991;11:201–230. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80184-7.
    1. Marcora S.M., Staiano W., Manning V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 2009;106:857–864. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008.
    1. Tsunetsugu Y., Park B.-J., Ishii H., Hirano H., Kagawa T., Miyazaki Y. Physiological effects of shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest) in an old-growth broadleaf forest in yamagata prefecture, Japan. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2007;26:135–142. doi: 10.2114/jpa2.26.135.
    1. Park B.-J., Tsunetsugu Y., Ishii H., Furuhashi S., Hirano H., Kagawa T., Miyazaki Y. Physiological effects of shinrin-yoku (taking in the atmosphere of the forest) in a mixed forest in shinano town, Japan. Scand. J. For. Res. 2008;23:278–283. doi: 10.1080/02827580802055978.
    1. Tucker R. The anticipatory regulation of performance: The physiological basis for pacing strategies and the development of a perception-based model for exercise performance. Brit. J. Sports Med. 2009;43:392–400. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2008.050799.
    1. Hampson D.B., Gibson A.S.C., Lambert M.I., Noakes T.D. The influence of sensory cues on the perception of exertion during exercise and central regulation of exercise performance. Sports Med. 2001;31:935–952. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200131130-00004.
    1. Reilly T., Baxter C. Influence of time of day on reactions to cycling at a fixed high intensity. Brit. J. Sport. Med. 1983;17:128–130. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.17.2.128.
    1. Bruce R., Blackmon J., Jones J., Strait G. Exercising testing in adult normal subjects and cardiac patients. Pediatrics. 1963;32:742–756. doi: 10.1111/j.1542-474X.2004.93003.x.
    1. Bruce R., Blackmon J., Jones J., Strait G. Exercising testing in adult normal subjects and cardiac patients *. Ann. Noninvasive Electrocardiol. 2004;9:291–303. doi: 10.1111/j.1542-474X.2004.93003.x.
    1. Folstein M.F., Folstein S.E., McHugh P.R. “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.
    1. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Psychological Corp.; Pasig City, Philippine: 1955.
    1. Cowan N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav. Brain Sci. 2001;24:87–114. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01003922.
    1. Taylor A.F., Kuo F.E. Children with attention deficits concentrate better after walk in the park. J. Atten. Disord. 2009;12:402–409. doi: 10.1177/1087054708323000.
    1. Stark M.A. Directed attention in normal and high-risk pregnancy. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 2006;35:241–249. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2006.00035.x.
    1. Taylor A.F., Kuo F.E., Sullivan W.C. Views of nature and self-discipline: Evidence from inner city children. J. Environ. Psychol. 2002;22:49–63. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2001.0241.
    1. Goyal M.R. Biomechanics of Artificial Organs and Prostheses. CRC Press; Boca Raton, FL, USA: 2014.
    1. IBM Corp . IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 19.0. IBM Corp.; Armonk, NY, USA: 2010.
    1. Bowler D.E., Buyung-Ali L.M., Knight T.M., Pullin A.S. A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health. 2010;10 doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-456.
    1. Barton J., Hine R., Pretty J. The health benefits of walking in greenspaces of high natural and heritage value. J. Integr. Environ. Sci. 2009;6:261–278. doi: 10.1080/19438150903378425.
    1. Li Q., Otsuka T., Kobayashi M., Wakayama Y., Inagaki H., Katsumata M., Hirata Y., Li Y., Hirata K., Shimizu T. Acute effects of walking in forest environments on cardiovascular and metabolic parameters. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2011;111:2845–2853. doi: 10.1007/s00421-011-1918-z.
    1. Fillingim R.B., Fine M.A. The effects of internal versus external information processing on symptom perception in an exercise setting. Health Psychol. 1986;5:115–123. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.5.2.115.
    1. Lohse K.R., Sherwood D.E. Defining the focus of attention: Effects of attention on perceived exertion and fatigue. Front. Psychol. 2011;2 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00332.
    1. Kuo F.E., Taylor A.F. A potential natural treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from a national study. Amer. J. Public Health. 2004;94 doi: 10.2105/AJPH.94.9.1580.
    1. Barton J., Griffin M., Pretty J. Exercise-, nature-and socially interactive-based initiatives improve mood and self-esteem in the clinical population. Perspect. Public Health. 2012;132:89–96. doi: 10.1177/1757913910393862.

Source: PubMed

3
Prenumerera