Mechanisms underlying age- and performance-related differences in working memory

Kirk R Daffner, Hyemi Chong, Xue Sun, Elise C Tarbi, Jenna L Riis, Scott M McGinnis, Phillip J Holcomb, Kirk R Daffner, Hyemi Chong, Xue Sun, Elise C Tarbi, Jenna L Riis, Scott M McGinnis, Phillip J Holcomb

Abstract

This study took advantage of the subsecond temporal resolution of ERPs to investigate mechanisms underlying age- and performance-related differences in working memory. Young and old subjects participated in a verbal n-back task with three levels of difficulty. Each group was divided into high and low performers based on accuracy under the 2-back condition. Both old subjects and low-performing young subjects exhibited impairments in preliminary mismatch/match detection operations (indexed by the anterior N2 component). This may have undermined the quality of information available for the subsequent decision-making process (indexed by the P3 component), necessitating the appropriation of more resources. Additional anterior and right hemisphere activity was recruited by old subjects. Neural efficiency and the capacity to allocate more resources to decision-making differed between high and low performers in both age groups. Under low demand conditions, high performers executed the task utilizing fewer resources than low performers (indexed by the P3 amplitude). As task requirements increased, high-performing young and old subjects were able to appropriate additional resources to decision-making, whereas their low-performing counterparts allocated fewer resources. Higher task demands increased utilization of processing capacity for operations other than decision-making (e.g., sustained attention) that depend upon a shared pool of limited resources. As demands increased, all groups allocated additional resources to the process of sustaining attention (indexed by the posterior slow wave). Demands appeared to have exceeded capacity in low performers, leading to a reduction of resources available to the decision-making process, which likely contributed to a decline in performance.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Illustration of the timing of the stimulus presentation under the 2-back condition.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Montage illustrating the location of electrode sites, based on the International 10–20 System, which includes midline, two inner lateral, and two outer lateral columns, each with seven anteroposterior sites.
Figure 3
Figure 3
(A) Accuracy level (% hits – % false alarms) to n-back targets under each condition. (B) RT (msec) to n-back targets under each condition.
Figure 4
Figure 4
Midline grand-average ERP plots of n-back hits. Arrows indicate N2, P3, and posterior slow wave components.
Figure 5
Figure 5
N2 surface potential maps in response to nontarget (mismatch) events under each condition.
Figure 6
Figure 6
P3 surface potential maps in response to n-back hits under each condition.
Figure 7
Figure 7
Line graph illustrating the midline P3 amplitudes (mean of the 7 electrode sites) in response to n-back hits under each condition.
Figure 8
Figure 8
Line graph illustrating the mean posterior slow wave amplitudes at Cz–Pz electrode sites under each condition.

References

    1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.
    1. Benton AL, Varney NR, Hamsher KdeS, Spreen O. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1983.
    1. Beste C, Willemssen R, Saft C, Falkenstein M. Error processing in normal aging and in basal ganglia disorders. Neuroscience. 2009;159:143–149.
    1. Breton F, Ritter W, Simson R, Vaughan HG., Jr The N2 component elicited by stimulus matches and multiple targets. Biological Psychology. 1988;27:23–44.
    1. Brumback CR, Low KA, Gratton G, Fabiani M. Putting things into perspective: Individual differences in working-memory span and the integration of information. Experimental Psychology. 2005;52:21–30.
    1. Cabeza R, Anderson ND, Locantore JK, McIntosh AR. Aging gracefully: Compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage. 2002;17:1394–1402.
    1. Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Finn K, Coppola R, Frank JA, et al. Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. Cerebral Cortex. 1999;9:20–26.
    1. Charlton RA, Barrick TR, McIntyre DJ, Shen Y, O’Sullivan M, Howe FA, et al. White matter damage on diffusion tensor imaging correlates with age-related cognitive decline. Neurology. 2006;66:217–222.
    1. Daffner KR. Promoting successful cognitive aging: A comprehensive review. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease. 2010;19:1101–1122.
    1. Daffner KR, Ryan KK, Williams DM, Budson AE, Rentz DM, Wolk DA, et al. Increased responsiveness to novelty is associated with successful cognitive aging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18:1759–1773.
    1. Dale AM. Source localization and spatial discriminant analysis of event-related potentials: Linear approaches (brain cortical surface) Dissertation Abstracts International. 1994;55-07B:2559.
    1. Daselaar SM, Cabeza R. Age-related changes in hemispheric organization. In: Cabeza R, Nyberg L, Park D, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of aging. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 325–353.
    1. Davis SW, Dennis NA, Daselaar SM, Fleck MS, Cabeza R. Que PASA? The posterior anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:1201–1209.
    1. Dobbs AR, Rule BG. Adult age differences in working memory. Psychology and Aging. 1989;4:500–503.
    1. Donchin E. Surprise!… surprise? Psychophysiology. 1981;18:493–513.
    1. Donchin E, Coles MGH. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1988;11:357–374.
    1. Egan V, Chiswick A, Santosh C, Naidu K, Remmington J, Best J. Size isn’t everything: A study of brain volume, intelligence and auditory evoked potentials. Personality and Individual Differences. 1994;17:357–367.
    1. Fabiani M, Friedman D, Cheng JC. Individual differences in P3 scalp distribution in older adults, and their relationship to frontal lobe function. Psychophysiology. 1998;35:698–708.
    1. Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Amlien I, Espeseth T, Reinvang I, Raz N, et al. High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:2001–2012.
    1. Folstein JR, Van Petten C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:152–170.
    1. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198.
    1. Ford JM. Does P300 reflect template match/mismatch? In: Otto DA, editor. Multidisciplinary perspectives in event-related brain potential research. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 1978. pp. 181–183.
    1. Gevins A, Smith ME, Le J, Leong H, Bennett J, Martin N, et al. High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human working memory. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1996;98:327–348.
    1. Goldberg TE, Berman KF, Fleming K, Ostrem J, Van Horn JD, Esposito G, et al. Uncoupling cognitive workload and prefrontal cortical physiology: A PET rCBF study. Neuroimage. 1998;7:296–303.
    1. Gopher D. On the psychophysics of workload: Why bother with subjective measures? Human Factors. 1984;26:519–532.
    1. Grady CL. Functional brain imaging and age-related changes in cognition. Biological Psychology. 2000;54:259–281.
    1. Hartley AA. Attention. In: Criak FIM, Salthouse TA, editors. The handbook of aging and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1992. pp. 3–49.
    1. Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Smith GE, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC. Neuropsychological tests’ norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE Token, WRAT-R Reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and JLO. Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1996;10:262–278.
    1. Jausovec N, Jausovec K. Correlations between ERP parameters and intelligence: A reconsideration. Biological Psychology. 2000;55:137–154.
    1. Johnson R. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology. 1986;23:367–384.
    1. Kahneman D. Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1973.
    1. Klingberg T, O’Sullivan BT, Roland PE. Bilateral activation of fronto-parietal networks by incrementing demand in a working memory task. Cerebral Cortex. 1997;7:465–471.
    1. Knight RT. Distributed cortical network for visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1997;9:75–91.
    1. Kok A. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:557–577.
    1. Kok A, de Jong HL. The effect of repetition of infrequent familiar and unfamiliar visual patterns on components of the event-related brain potential. Biological Psychology. 1980;10:167–188.
    1. Kotchoubey BI, Jordan JS, Grozinger B, Westphal KP, Kornhuber HH. Event-related brain potentials in a varied-set memory search task: A reconsideration. Psychophysiology. 1996;33:530–540.
    1. Kutas M, McCarthy G, Donchin E. Augmenting mental chronometry: The P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. Science. 1977;197:792–795.
    1. Lovrich D, Simson R, Vaughan HG, Jr, Ritter W. Topography of visual event-related potentials during geometric and phonetic discriminations. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1986;65:1–12.
    1. Luck SJ. An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2005.
    1. Mathewson KJ, Dywan J, Snyder PJ, Tays WJ, Segalowitz SJ. Aging and electrocortical response to error feedback during a spatial learning task. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:936–948.
    1. Mattay VS, Fera F, Tessitore A, Hariri AR, Berman KF, Das S, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory capacity. Neuroscience Letters. 2006;392:32–37.
    1. McEvoy LK, Smith ME, Gevins A. Dynamic cortical networks of verbal and spatial working memory: Effects of memory load and task practice. Cerebral Cortex. 1998;8:563–574.
    1. McGarry-Roberts P, Stelmack R, Campbell K. Intelligence, reaction time, and event-related potentials. Intelligence. 1992;16:289–313.
    1. Mecklinger A, Kramer AF, Strayer DL. Event related potentials and EEG components in a semantic memory search task. Psychophysiology. 1992;29:104–119.
    1. Mesulam M. Behavioral neuroanatomy: Large scale networks, association cortex, frontal syndromes, the limbic system and hemispheric specializations. In: Mesulam M, editor. Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 1–120.
    1. Muller NG, Knight RT. Age-related changes in fronto-parietal networks during spatial memory: An ERP study. Brain Research, Cognitive Brain Research. 2002;13:221–234.
    1. Oberauer K. Binding and inhibition in working memory: Individual and age differences in short-term recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2005;134:368–387.
    1. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. n-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;25:46–59.
    1. Polich J. Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. Psychophysiology. 1996;33:334–353.
    1. Polich J, Howard L, Starr A. P300 latency correlates with digit span. Psychophysiology. 1983;20:665–669.
    1. Pritchard WS, Shappell SA, Brandt ME. Psychophysiology of N200/N400: A review and classification scheme. In: Jennings JR, Ackles PK, Coles MG, editors. Advances in psychophysiology. Vol. 4. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 1991. pp. 43–106.
    1. Raz N, Gunning-Dixon FM, Head D, Dupuis JH, Acker JD. Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive aging: Evidence from structural magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:95–114.
    1. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA. Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17:177–182.
    1. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Jonides J, Smith EE, Hartley A, Miller A, Marshuetz C, et al. Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12:174–187.
    1. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Sylvester C-YC. The cognitive neuroscience of working memory and aging. In: Cabeza R, Nyberg L, Park D, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral aging. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 186–217.
    1. Riis JL, Chong H, McGinnnis S, Tarbi E, Sun X, Holcomb PJ, et al. Age-related changes in early novelty processing as measured by ERPs. Biological Psychology. 2009;82:33–44.
    1. Riis JL, Chong H, Ryan KK, Wolk DA, Rentz DM, Holcomb PJ, et al. Compensatory neural activity distinguishes different patterns of normal cognitive aging. Neuroimage. 2008;39:441–454.
    1. Ruchkin DS, Johnson R, Jr, Canoune H, Ritter W. Short-term memory storage and retention: An event-related brain potential study. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1990;76:419–439.
    1. Ruchkin DS, Johnson R, Jr, Grafman J, Canoune H, Ritter W. Distinctions and similarities among working memory processes: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, Cognitive Brain Research. 1992;1:53–66.
    1. Ruchkin DS, Sutton S. Positive slow wave and P300: Association and disassociation. In: Gaillard AWK, Ritter W, editors. Tutorials in ERP research: Endogenous components. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing; 1983. pp. 233–250.
    1. Ruchkin DS, Sutton S, Kietzman ML, Silver K. Slow wave and P300 in signal detection. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1980;50:35–47.
    1. Rugg MD, Morcom AM. The relationship between brain activity, cognitive performance, and aging. In: Cabeza R, Nyberg L, Park D, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Linking cognitive and cerebral aging. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 132–154.
    1. Rushby JA, Barry RJ, Doherty RJ. Separation of the components of the late positive complex in an ERP dishabituation paradigm. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2005;116:2363–2380.
    1. Ryan J, Paolo A. A screening procedure for estimating premorbid intelligence in the elderly. Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1992;6:53–62.
    1. Rypma B, D’Esposito M. The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: Effects of memory load and individual differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 1999;96:6558–6563.
    1. Rypma B, D’Esposito M. Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in human working memory. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:509–515.
    1. Salat DH, Lee SY, van der Kouwe AJ, Greve DN, Fischl B, Rosas HD. Age-associated alterations in cortical gray and white matter signal intensity and gray to white matter contrast. Neuroimage. 2009;48:21–28.
    1. Salthouse TA. Resource-reduction interpretations of cognitive aging. Developmental Review. 1988:238–272.
    1. Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review. 1996;103:403–428.
    1. Salthouse TA, Ferrer-Caja E. What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables? Psychology and Aging. 2003;18:91–110.
    1. Schneider-Garces NJ, Gordon BA, Brumback-Peltz CR, Shin E, Lee Y, Sutton BP, et al. Span, CRUNCH, and beyond: Working memory capacity and the aging brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;22:655–669.
    1. Schultz SK, O’Leary DS, Boles Ponto LL, Watkins GL, Hichwa RD, Andreasen NC. Age-related changes in regional cerebral blood flow among young to mid-life adults. NeuroReport. 1999;10:2493–2496.
    1. Sirevaag EJ, Kramer AF, Coles MG, Donchin E. Resource reciprocity: An event-related brain potentials analysis. Acta Psychologica. 1989;70:77–97.
    1. Smith EE, Geva A, Jonides J, Miller A, Reuter-Lorenz P, Koeppe RA. The neural basis of task-switching in working memory: Effects of performance and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 2001;98:2095–2100.
    1. Snyder E, Hillyard SA. Changes in visual event-related potentials in older persons. In: Hoffmeister F, Muller C, editors. Bayer Symposium VII: Brain function in old age. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1979. pp. 112–125.
    1. Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.
    1. Squires KC, Hillyard SA, Lindsay PH. Vertex potentials evoked during auditory signal detection: Relation to decision criteria. Perception & Psychophysics. 1973;14:25–31.
    1. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2015–2028.
    1. Themanson JR, Hillman CH, Curtin JJ. Age and physical activity influences on action monitoring during task switching. Neurobiology of Aging. 2006;27:1335–1345.
    1. Tombaugh TN. Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2004;19:203–214.
    1. Urbach TP, Kutas M. The intractability of scaling scalp distributions to infer neuroelectric sources. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:791–808.
    1. Vaidya JG, Paradiso S, Boles Ponto LL, McCormick LM, Robinson RG. Aging, grey matter, and blood flow in the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1346–1353.
    1. Walhovd KB, Fjell AM. The relationship between P3 and neuropsychological function in an adult life span sample. Biological Psychology. 2002;62:65–87.
    1. Wang Y, Tian S, Wang H, Cui L, Zhang Y, Zhang X. Event-related potentials evoked by multi-feature conflict under different attentive conditions. Experimental Brain Research. 2003;148:451–457.
    1. Watter S, Geffen GM, Geffen LB. The n-back as a dual-task: P300 morphology under divided attention. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:998–1003.
    1. Wickens C, Kramer A, Vanasse L, Donchin E. Performance of concurrent tasks: A psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. Science. 1983;221:1080–1082.
    1. Willemssen R, Falkenstein M, Schwarz M, Muller T, Beste C. Effects of aging, Parkinson’s disease, and dopaminergic medication on response selection and control. Neurobiology of Aging in press.

Source: PubMed

3
Prenumerera