Vagus nerve stimulation promotes generalization of conditioned fear extinction and reduces anxiety in rats

Lindsey J Noble, Venkat B Meruva, Seth A Hays, Robert L Rennaker, Michael P Kilgard, Christa K McIntyre, Lindsey J Noble, Venkat B Meruva, Seth A Hays, Robert L Rennaker, Michael P Kilgard, Christa K McIntyre

Abstract

Background: Exposure-based therapies are used to treat a variety of trauma- and anxiety-related disorders by generating successful extinction following cue exposure during treatment. The development of adjuvant strategies that accelerate extinction learning, improve tolerability, and increase efficiency of treatment could increase the efficacy of exposure-based therapies. Vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) paired with exposure can enhance fear extinction, in rat models of psychiatric disorders, and chronic administration of VNS reduces anxiety in rats and humans.

Objective: We tested whether VNS, like other cognitive enhancers, could produce generalization of extinction for stimuli that are not presented during the extinction sessions, but are associated with the fear event.

Methods: Male Sprague Dawley rats underwent auditory fear conditioning with two easily discriminable auditory stimuli. Following fear conditioning, extinction training consisted of exposure to only one of the conditioned sounds. Half of the rats received VNS and half received sham stimulation during with sound presentations. VNS effects on anxiety were examined in a separate study where VNS was administered prior to testing on the elevated plus maze.

Results: Sham stimulated rats given 20 presentations of a conditioned stimulus (CS) during the extinction session showed performance that was matched to VNS-treated rats given only 4 presentations of the CS. Despite comparable levels of freezing to the presented CS, only the VNS-treated rats showed a significant decrease in freezing to the CS that was not presented. VNS-induced generalization of extinction was observed only when the two sounds were paired with footshock within the same fear conditioning session; VNS did not promote generalization of extinction when the two sounds were conditioned on different days or in different contexts. On the anxiety test, VNS administration significantly increased time spent in the open arms of the elevated plus maze.

Conclusion: These results provide evidence that VNS can promote generalization of extinction to other stimuli associated with a specific fear experience. Furthermore, non-contingent VNS appears to reduce anxiety. The ability to generalize extinction and reduce anxiety makes VNS a potential candidate for use as an adjunctive strategy to improve the efficacy and tolerability of exposure-based therapies.

Keywords: Anxiety; Extinction; Fear; Generalization; VNS; Vagus.

Conflict of interest statement

Conflict of interest:

This work has not been published and has not been submitted for publication elsewhere while under consideration. Authors: Noble, Meruva, and Hays declare no potential conflicts of interest. Dr. Kilgard is a paid consultant for and shareholder of MicroTransponder. Drs. Kilgard and McIntyre are authors of a patent entitled “Enhancing Fear Extinction using Vagus Nerve Stimulation”. Dr. Rennaker is an owner of Vulintis Inc. and Optokinetics and is a paid consultant for Konan Medical USA; none of these financial interests are related to this work.

Copyright © 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Figures

Figure 1.. Spectrograms of each conditioned stimulus…
Figure 1.. Spectrograms of each conditioned stimulus used during AFC. A. Spectrogram for CS1 (machine gun fire). B. Spectrogram for CS2 (marmoset vocalization).
Spectrograms for CS1 and CS2 were used in generalization experiments, as they are easy to discriminate given they have distinct differences in frequency bandwidth, frequency modulation, rise time, and decay time. Duration of stimuli were also different, CS1 lasted 50 seconds while CS2 lasted 39 seconds.
Figure 2.. VNS+Extinction leads to generalization of…
Figure 2.. VNS+Extinction leads to generalization of extinction for both the CS that was presented and the CS that was not presented during extinction training. A. Timeline for AFC, conditioned fear response testing, and extinction treatment.
AFC was administered across two days where both CS1 and CS2 were presented and paired with footshocks on each day. The order of CS presentation was random and interleaved such that each rat was administered 4 presentations of each CS on each day. Following AFC, rats underwent a pre-extinction conditioned fear response test (CFRT) where they were presented with four presentations of each CS to measure conditioned fear. Twenty-four hours later, rats underwent treatment consisting of either: 20 extinction trials of only CS1 or only CS2, paired with sham stimulation (Extended Extinction), 4 extinction trials of only CS1 or only CS2, paired with VNS (VNS+Extinction), or no exposures to either CS but equivalent amounts of VNS in the home cage (VNS Alone). A day later, rats underwent another CFRT to assess levels of conditioned fear to both stimuli. B. Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction rats show equal reduction in conditioned fear to the Presented CS. Following 20 unreinforced presentations of a CS, Extended Extinction rats showed reduced freezing in response to the CS versus freezing during Pre-Extinction (p=2.3×10−4). VNS+Extinction rats also showed reduced freezing in response to the Presented CS versus Pre-Extinction (p=3.2×10−4). C. Only VNS+Extinction leads to generalization of extinction for the Non-Presented CS. Extended Extinction did not show reduced fear in response to the Non-Presented CS versus the Pre-Extinction freezing response (p=0.15). In contrast, VNS+Extinction rats showed a reduction in freezing response to presentation of the Non-Presented CS versus the Pre-Extinction freezing response, even though it was never presented during extinction (p=2.1×10−4). D. VNS Alone does not lead to extinction of either CS. Following VNS in the home cage in lieu of extinction, VNS Alone rats showed no reduction in conditioned fear for either CS versus the Pre-Extinction CFRT (p=0.16). E. Context extinction cannot explain VNS+Extinction generalization. There is no difference in baseline fear to the context between Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction groups, rats spend equal amounts of time freezing to the context during the Post-Extinction CFRT (p=0.68). VNS Alone rats show elevated freezing to the context versus Extended Extinction (p=2.1×10−3) and VNS+Extinction rats (p=3.8×10−3).
Figure 3.. Separating CS1 and CS2 conditioning…
Figure 3.. Separating CS1 and CS2 conditioning by twenty-four hours blocks the VNS effect on generalization. A. Timeline for AFC, CFRT, and extinction treatment.
AFC was administered across two days where CS1 was presented on 1 day and CS2 was presented twenty-four hours later. A CFRT to CS1 was administered on day 3 where CS1 was presented four times without reinforcement to measure conditioned fear to CS1. The next day, a CFRT was administered to CS2 where CS2 was presented four times without reinforcement to measure conditioned fear to CS2. Twenty-four hours later, rats underwent extinction consisting of either: 20 extinction trials of CS1 or CS2 paired with sham stimulation (Extended Extinction), or 4 extinction trials of CS1 or CS2 paired with VNS (VNS+Extinction). Following extinction, a Post-Extinction CFRT was administered where four presentations of each CS were presented and randomly interleaved to assess conditioned fear to each CS following extinction. B. Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction rats show equal reduction in conditioned fear to the Presented CS. Following 20 presentations of the Presented CS, both Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction rats showed reduced freezing to the Presented CS (p=7.1×10−3 vs PreExtinction) and (p=4.4×10−3 vs. Pre-Extinction), respectively. C. Neither Extended Extinction rats or VNS+Extinction rats show reduced freezing to the Non-Presented CS. After extinction training, freezing to the Non-Presented CS was not reduced for Extra Extinction rats (p=0.12). In contrast to Figure 1, freezing to the Non-Presented CS was not reduced in (p=0.29). D. Baseline freezing to the context is not different between groups. Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction rats spent equivalent amounts of time freezing to the extinction context during the five minutes prior to CS presentation at the Post-Extinction CFRT (p=0.55).
Figure 4.. Separating conditioning of CS1 and…
Figure 4.. Separating conditioning of CS1 and CS2 by context blocks the VNS+Extinction effect on generalization. A. Timeline for AFC, CFRT, and extinction treatment.
AFC was administered across two days, where CS1 was presented only in Context A, and CS2 was presented only in Context C, but both CS1 and CS2 were presented during the same session. Twenty-four hours after AFC, a Pre-Extinction CFRT was administered for CS1 in Context A, where CS1 was presented four times without reinforcement. On the same day, a Pre-Extinction CFRT was also administered to CS2 in Context C, where CS2 was presented four times without reinforcement. Following the Pre-Extinction CFRT day, rats were subjected to extinction treatment consisting of either: 20 presentations of CS1 in Context A paired with sham stimulation (Extended Extinction), or 4 presentations of CS1 in Context A paired with VNS (VNS+Extinction). Twenty-four hours following extinction, a Post-Extinction CFRT was given to CS1 in Context A, and then on the same day a Post-Extinction CFRT was given for CS2 in Context C. B. Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction rats show equal reduction in conditioned fear to the Presented CS. Following extinction of CS1 in Context A, Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction rats showed reduced conditioned fear to the Presented CS versus Pre-Extinction (p=2.3×10−3) and (p=1.2×10−3), respectively. C. Neither Extended Extinction rats or VNS+Extinction rats show reduced freezing to the Non-Presented CS. Freezing to the Non-Presented CS was not reduced after Extinction training for Extended Extinction rats (p=0.38). Similar to Figure 2, freezing to the Non-Presented CS was also not reduced for VNS+Extinction rats versus Pre-Extinction (p=0.40). D. Baseline freezing to the extinction context is not different between groups. Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction rats spent an equal amount of time freezing to the extinction context during the five minutes prior to presentation of CS1 during the Post-Extinction CFRT (p=0.53). E. Baseline freezing to the non-extinction context is not different between groups. Extended Extinction and VNS+Extinction rats spent an equal amount of time freezing to the non-extinction context during the five minutes prior to CS2 presentation during the Post-Extinction CFRT (p=0.62).
Figure 5.. VNS reduces anxiety.
Figure 5.. VNS reduces anxiety.
Rats were given one non-contingent train of VNS or sham stimulation in their home cage. Ten minutes later, they were removed from their home cage and placed on the elevated plus maze (EPM). VNS-treated rats spent significantly more time in the open arms of the EPM when compared to sham-treated rats (p=2.1×10−3), indicating a VNS-induced reduction in anxiety. The percent of time spent moving during EPM testing was similar between groups, indicating no general locomotor effect.
Figure 6.. Dual benefits of VNS.
Figure 6.. Dual benefits of VNS.
Cognitive enhancers show promise as adjuncts to exposurebased therapies; however, most pharmaceutical enhancers of cognition do not improve tolerability of the therapy by reducing anxiety. Anxiolytic drugs may improve tolerability but they are not effective, possibly because they interfere with the consolidation of new extinction memories. However, VNS enhances memory consolidation and accelerates extinction, and it also reduces anxiety. This unique combination of effects suggests that VNS may offer a desirable alternative to drugs as an adjunct therapy.

References

    1. Rauch S, Foa E (2006) Emotional processing theory (EPT) and exposure therapy for PTSD. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy 36: 1–7.
    1. Rosen CS, Chow HC, Finney JF, Greenbaum MA, Moos RH, Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA (2004) VA practice patterns and practice guidelines for treating posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of traumatic stress 17:213–222.
    1. Rothbaum BO, Hodges L, Watson BA, Kessler GD, Opdyke D (1999) Virtual reality exposure therapy for PTSD Vietnam veterans: A case study. Behaviour Research and Therapy 34: 477–481.
    1. Kushner MG, et al., (2007) D-cycloserine augmented exposure therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 62: 835–838
    1. Foa EB, et al., (2005) Randomized, placebo-controlled trial of exposure and ritual prevention, clomipramine, and their combination in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 162: 151–161.
    1. Riggs DS, Foa EB (1993) Obsessive-compulsive disorder In: Barlow DH, editor. Clinical Handbook of Psychological Disorders, New York: Guilford Press, 189–239.
    1. Rothbaum BO, Hodges L, Watson BA, Kessler GD, Opdyke D (1996) Virtual reality exposure therapy in the treatment of fear of flying: A case report. Behaviour Research Therapy 34: 477–481.
    1. Wiederhold BK, Jang DP, Gevritz RG, Kim SI, Kim IY, Wiederhold MD (2002) The treatment of fear of flying: a controlled study of imaginal and virtual reality graded exposure therapy. IEEE Transactions on Information 6: 1–10.
    1. Bouton ME (2004) Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & memory 11:485– 494.
    1. Sotres-Bayon F, Cain CK, LeDoux JE (2006) Brain mechanisms of fear extinction: historical perspectives on the contribution of prefrontal cortex. Biological Psychiatry 60: 329–336.
    1. Quirk GJ, Mueller D (2008) Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 56–72.
    1. Schottenbauer MA, Glass CR, Arnkoff DB, Tendick V, Gray SH (2008) Nonresponse and dropout rates in outcome studies on PTSD: review and methodological considerations. Psychiatry 71: 134–68.
    1. Powers MB, Halpern JM, Ferenschak MP, Gillihan SJ, Foa EB (2010) A meta-analytic review of prolonged exposure for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology Review 30: 635–641.
    1. Davis M, Myers KM, Chhatwal J, Ressler KJ (2006) Pharmacological treatments that facilitate extinction of fear: relevance to psychotherapy. NeuroRx 3: 82–96.
    1. Milad MR, Orr SP, Lasko NB, Chang Y, Rauch SL, Pitman RK (2008) Presence and acquired origin of reduced recall for fear extinction in PTSD: results of a twin study. J Psychiatr Res 42: 515– 20.
    1. Milad MR et al., (2009) Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 66: 1075–1082.
    1. Rothbaum BO, Davis M (2003) Applying learning principles to the treatment of post-trauma reactions. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1008: 112–21.
    1. Milad MR, et al., (2013) Deficits in conditioned fear extinction in obsessive-compulsive disorder and neurobiological changes in the fear circuit. JAMA Psychiatry 70: 608–618.
    1. Powers MB, Smits J, Otto MW, Sanders C, Emmelkamp PM (2009) Facilitation of fear extinction in phobic participants with a novel cognitive enhancer: a randomized placebo controlled trial of yohimbine augmentation. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23:350–356.
    1. McNally RJ (1996) Perceptual implicit memory for trauma-related information in posttraumatic stress disorder. Cognition and Emotion 10: 551–556.
    1. McNally RJ (1997) Implicit and explicit memory for trauma-related information in PTSD. Annals of the Academy of Sciences 821: 219–224.
    1. Byrne SP, Rapee RM, Richardson R, Malhi GS, Jones M, Hudson JL (2015) D-cycloserine enhances generalization of fear extinction in children. Depression and Anxiety 32: 408–414.
    1. Drexler MS, Hamacher-Dang TC, Wolf OT (2017) Stress before extinction learning enhances and generalizes extinction memory in a predictive learning task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 141: 143–149.
    1. Mataiz-Cols et al., (2017) D-cycloserine augmentation of exposure-based cognitive behavior therapy for anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, and posttraumatic stress disorders. A systematic review and meta-analysis of individual patient data. JAMA Psychiatry 74: 501–510.
    1. Litz BT, Salters-Pedneault K, Steenkamp M, Hermos JA, Bryant RA, Otto MW (2012) A randomized placebo-controlled trial of d-cycloserine and exposure therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal Psychiatry Research 46: 1184–90.
    1. Clark KB, Krahl S, Smith DC, Jensen RA (1995) Post-training unilateral vagal stimulation enhances retention performance in the rat. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 63:213–216.
    1. Clark KB, Narikotu DK, Smith DC, Browning RA, Jensen RA (1999) Enhanced recognition memory following vagus nerve stimulation in human subjects. Nature Neuroscience 2:94–98.
    1. Alvarez-Dieppa AC, Griffin K, Cavalier S, McIntyre CK (2016) Vagus nerve stimulation enhances extinction of conditioned fear in rats and modulates Arc protein, CaMKII, and GluN2Bcontaining NMDA receptors in the basolateral amygdala. Neural Plasticity 1–11.
    1. Peña DF, Engineer ND, McIntyre CK (2013) Rapid remission of conditioned fear expression with extinction training paired with vagus nerve stimulation. Biol Psychiatry 73: 1071–7.
    1. Peña DF, Childs JE, Willett S, Vital A, McIntyre CK, Kroener S (2014) Vagus nerve stimulation enhances extinction of conditioned fear and modulates plasticity in the pathway from the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to the amygdala. Front Behav Neurosci 8: 327.
    1. Noble LJ, Gonzalez IJ, Meruva VB, Callahan KA, Belfort BD, Ramananthan KR, Meyers E, Kilgard MP, Rennaker RL, McIntyre CK (2017) Effects of vagus nerve stimulation on extinction of conditioned fear and post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms in rats. Translational Psychiatry 7: 1–8.
    1. Shah AP, Carreno FR, Wu H, Chung YA, Frazer A (2016) Role of TrkB in the anxiolytic-like and antidepressant-like effects of vagal nerve stimulation: Comparison with desipramine. Neuroscience 322: 273–286.
    1. Labiner DM & Ahern GL (2007) Vagus nerve stimulation therapy in depression and epilepsy: therapeutic parameter settings. Acta Neurol Scand 115:23–33
    1. George MS, Ward HE, Ninan PT, Pollack M, Nahas Z, Anderson B, Kose S, Howland RH, Goodman WK, Ballenger JC (2008) A pilot study of vagus nerve stimulation (VNS) for treatmentresistant anxiety disorders. Brain Stimulation 1:112–21.
    1. Childs JE, Alvarez-Dieppa AC, McIntyre CK, Kroener S (2015) Vagus nerve stimulation as a tool to induce plasticity in pathways relevant for extinction learning. Journal of Visual Exploration: 53032.
    1. George MS, Sackeim HA, Rush AJ, Marangell LB, Nahas Z, Husain MM, et al., (2000) Vagus nerve stimulation: a new tool for brain research and therapy. Biological Psychiatry 47: 287–95.
    1. Engineer CT, Perez CA, Chen YH, Carraway RS, Reed AC, Shetake JA, Jakkamsetti V, Chang KQ, Kilgard MP (2008) Cortical activity patterns predict speech discrimination ability. Nat Neurosci 11(5):603–8.
    1. Engineer CT, Perez CA, Carraway RS, Chang KQ, Roland JL, Sloan AM, Kilgard MP. Similarity of cortical activity patterns predicts generalization behavior. 2013, PLoS One. 8(10):e78607.
    1. Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M (1985) Validation of open:closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J Neuroscience Methods 14: 149–87.
    1. Rosen JB, Schulkin J (1998) From normal fear to pathological anxiety. Psychological Review 105: 325–350.
    1. Liu X, Ramirez S, Pang PT, Puryear CB, Govindarajam A, Deisseroth K, Tonegowa S (2012) Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature 484: 381–385
    1. Cai D et al. (2016) A shared neural ensemble links distinct contextual memories encoded close in time. Nature 534:115–118
    1. Guzowski JF, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, Worley PF (1999) Environment-specific expression of the immediate-early gene Arc in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Nature 2:1120–1124.
    1. Bieszczad KM, Bechay K, Rusche JR, Jacques V, Kudugunti S, Miao W, Weinberger NM, McGaugh JL, Wood MA (2015) Histone deacetylase inhibition via RGFP966 releases the brakes on sensory cortical plasticity and the specificity of memory formation. Journal of Neuroscience 35: 13124–13132
    1. Engineer ND, Riley JR, Seale JD, Vrana WA, Shetake HA, Sudanagunta SP, Borland MS, Kilgard MP (2011) Reversing pathological neural activity using targeted plasticity. Nature; 470: 101–06.
    1. Hassert DL, Miyashita T, Williams CL (2004) The effects of peripheral vagal nerve stimulation at a memory-modulating intensity on norepinephrine output in the basolateral amygdala. Behavioral Neuroscience 118: 79–88.
    1. Hays SA, Khodaparast N, Sloan AM, Fayyaz T (2013) The bradykinesia assessment task: an automated method to measure forelimb speed in rodents. Journal of neuroscience 214: 52–61.
    1. Hulsey DR, Riley JR, Loerwald KW, Rennaker RL, Kilgard MP, Hays SA (2016) Parametric characterization of neural activity in the locus coeruleus in response to vagus nerve stimulation. Exp Neurol 289: 21–30.
    1. Khodaparast N, et al., (2013). Vagus nerve stimulation during rehabilitative training improves forelimb strength following ischemic stroke. Neurobiol Dis 60: 80–8.
    1. Kilgard MP (2012) Harnessing plasticity to understand learning and treat disease. Trends Neuroscience 35: 715–22.
    1. Manta S, Dong J, Debonnel G, Blier P (2009) Enhancement of the function of rat serotonin and norepinephrine neurons by sustained vagus nerve stimulation. J Psychiatry Neurosci 34: 272–80.
    1. Porter BA, Khodaparast N, Fayyaz T, Cheung RJ (2012) Repeatedly pairing vagus nerve stimulation with a movement reorganizes primary motor cortex. Cerebral Cortex 22: 2365–74.
    1. Roosevelt RW, Smith DC, Clough RW, Jensen RA, Browning RA (2006) Increased extracellular concentrations of norepinephrine in cortex and hippocampus following vagus nerve stimulation in the rat. Brain Res 1119: 124–32.
    1. Shetake JA, Engineer ND, Vrana WA, Wolf JT (2012) Pairing tone trains with vagus nerve stimulation induces temporal plasticity in auditory cortex. Experimental Neurology 233: 342–9.
    1. Berlau DJ, McGaugh JL (2006) Enhancement of extinction memory consolidation: the role of noradrenergic and GABAergic systems within the basolateral amygdala. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 86: 123–132.
    1. Follesa P, et al., (2007) Vagus nerve stimulation increases norepinephrine concentration and the gene expression of BDNF and bFGF in the rat brain. Brain Research 1179: 28–34.
    1. Bramham CR, Messaoudi E (2005) BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: the synaptic consolidation hypothesis. Progress in Neurobiology 76:99–125.
    1. Chhatwal JP, Stanek-Rattiner L, Davis M, Ressler KJ (2006) Amygdala BDNF signaling is required for consolidation but not encoding of extinction. Nature Neuroscience 9: 870–872.
    1. Ying S, Futter M, Rosenblum K, Webber MJ, Hunt SP, Bliss TVP, Bramham CR (2002) Brainderived neurotrophic factor induces long-term potentiation in intact adult hippocampus: Requirement for ERK activation coupled to CREB and upregulation of Arc synthesis. Journal of Neuroscience 22: 1532–1540.
    1. Furmaga H, Carreno FR, Frazer A (2012) Vagal nerve stimulation rapidly activates brain-derived neurotrophic factor receptor TrkB in rat brain. PloS ONE 7: 1–10.
    1. Peters J, Dieppa-Perea LM, Melendez LM, Quirk GJ (2010) Induction of fear extinction with hippocampal-infralimbic BDNF. Science 328: 1288–1290.
    1. Rosas-Vidal LE, Do-Monte FH, Sotres-Bayon F, Quirk GJ (2014) Hippocampal-prefrontal BDNF and memory for fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology 39:2161–2169.
    1. Englot DJ, Chang EF, Auguste KI (2011) Vagus nerve stimulation for epilepsy: a meta-analysis of efficacy and predictors of response. J Neurosurg; 115: 1248–55.

Source: PubMed

3
Prenumerera