Sleep loss causes social withdrawal and loneliness

Eti Ben Simon, Matthew P Walker, Eti Ben Simon, Matthew P Walker

Abstract

Loneliness and social isolation markedly increase mortality risk, and are linked to numerous mental and physical comorbidities, including sleep disruption. But does sleep loss causally trigger loneliness? Here, we demonstrate that a lack of sleep leads to a neural and behavioral phenotype of social withdrawal and loneliness; one that can be perceived by other members of society, and reciprocally, makes those societal members lonelier in return. We propose a model in which sleep loss instigates a propagating, self-reinforcing cycle of social separation and withdrawal.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Experimental design. a In-laboratory repeated measures counterbalanced design. From left to right, the degree of social separation participants wished to keep from approaching others was assessed using a real, in-person human approach task, followed by a computerized version, used during a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scan. At the end of each session, participants performed a recorded interview with open-ended questions, which was subsequently rated by independent judges, blind to study goal or sleep manipulation. b Online phase study design. Participants were asked to complete daily sleep logs for two consecutive nights tracking their habitual variations in sleep time. Following each sleep survey, participants completed a next-day questionnaire assessing social behavior and loneliness
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Behavioral results. a Significant increases in social distancing following sleep deprivation, relative to the sleep rested condition, for both the in-person (left) and computerized (right) social distance tasks (13.2% and 17.7% differences, respectively). b Night-to-night increases or decreases in sleep efficiency were associated with significant day-to-day increases and decreases in loneliness, respectively. *P ≤ 0.05; error bars reflect standard error of the mean
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
fMRI results of the in-lab study. a and b Significant clusters of activation within a priori regions of interest (ROIs) during human-relative-to-object approach, showing sleep-deprivation-related increases in activity in regions of the Near Space network (red), yet sleep-loss-related decreases in activity in regions of the Theory-of-Mind network (blue; all P < 0.05, FDR corrected). c Increased reactivity (human > object approach) within the Near Space network was positively associated with greater social distance separation following sleep deprivation. This relationship was not significant for activity in regions of the Theory-of-Mind network. **P < 0.005; error bars reflect standard error of the mean; gray lines reflect zero crossings. DIPS—dorsal intra-parietal sulcus, vPM—ventral premotor region, TPJ—temporal–parietal junction, SD—sleep deprivation, SR—sleep rested
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Ratings of independent judges, blind to the experimental conditions. a Change in social ratings of sleep-deprived participants (relative to their ratings in the sleep-rested condition) by independent judges (see Table S1). Following sleep deprivation, participants were rated as being significantly lonelier and less desirable to interact with. Judges also indicated feeling significantly lonelier themselves after watching videos of sleep-deprived participants. b Lonely ratings were positively associated with judges’ corresponding feeling of loneliness: the lonelier a judge rated an individual in a video, the lonelier they themselves sequentially felt. c Judges were significantly more likely to choose a sleep-rested participant to collaborate with compared to chance and significantly more likely to choose a sleep-deprived participant as their least favorite option for collaboration. d The sleep-deprivation increase in reactivity within the Near Space Network (human-only approach) of the experimental participants, correlated with the decreasing desire of judges to socially interact with these sleep-deprived individuals. This relationship was not significant for judges’ rating of perceived loneliness. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.005; correlations remain significant without each bottom outlier data point (Fig. 4b: R = 0.5, P < 0.05, Fig. 4d: R = −0.52, P < 0.05); error bars reflect standard error of the mean. SD sleep deprivation, SR sleep rested

References

    1. Cacioppo S, Capitanio JP, Cacioppo JT. Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychol. Bull. 2014;140:1464. doi: 10.1037/a0037618.
    1. Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE, Gable SL, Hilmert CJ, Lieberman MD. Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. Neuroimage. 2007;35:1601–1612. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038.
    1. Cacioppo JT, et al. Loneliness and health: potential mechanisms. Psychosom. Med. 2002;64:407–417. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200205000-00005.
    1. House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D. Social relationships and health. Science. 1988;241:540. doi: 10.1126/science.3399889.
    1. Berkman LF, Syme SL. Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: a nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1979;109:186–204. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112674.
    1. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2015;10:227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352.
    1. Pressman SD, et al. Loneliness, social network size, and immune response to influenza vaccination in college freshmen. Health Psychol. 2005;24:297. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.3.297.
    1. Wilson RS, et al. Loneliness and risk of alzheimer disease. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2007;64:234–240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234.
    1. Lau S, Gruen GE. The social stigma of loneliness: effect of target person’s and perceiver’s sex. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1992;18:182–189. doi: 10.1177/0146167292182009.
    1. Lawrence EW, John AB. Keeping one’s distance: the influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychol. Sci. 2008;19:302–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02084.x.
    1. Renshaw PD, Brown PJ. Loneliness in middle childhood: concurrent and longitudinal predictors. Child Dev. 1993;64:1271–1284. doi: 10.2307/1131339.
    1. Rubin, K. H., LeMare, L. J. & Lollis, S. Social withdrawal in childhood: developmental pathways to peer rejection. Peer Rejection Child. 217–249 (1990).
    1. Cheek JM, Buss AH. Shyness and sociability. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1981;41:330. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.41.2.330.
    1. Nurmi JE, Salmela-Aro K. Social strategies and loneliness: a prospective study. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1997;23:205–215. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00039-1.
    1. Kaushal N, Nair D, Gozal D, Ramesh V. Socially isolated mice exhibit a blunted homeostatic sleep response to acute sleep deprivation compared to socially paired mice. Brain Res. 2012;1454:65–79. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.03.019.
    1. Tavernier R, Willoughby T. Bidirectional associations between sleep (quality and duration) and psychosocial functioning across the university years. Dev. Psychol. 2014;50:674. doi: 10.1037/a0034258.
    1. Cacioppo JT, et al. Do lonely days invade the nights? Potential social modulation of sleep efficiency. Psychol. Sci. 2002;13:384–387. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00469.x.
    1. Smith SS, Kozak N, Sullivan KA. An investigation of the relationship between subjective sleep quality, loneliness and mood in an Australian sample: can daily routine explain the links? Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry. 2012;58:166–171. doi: 10.1177/0020764010387551.
    1. Carney CE, Edinger JD, Meyer B, Lindman L, Istre T. Daily activities and sleep quality in college students. Chronobiol. Int. 2006;23:623–637. doi: 10.1080/07420520600650695.
    1. Perry A, Nichiporuk N, Knight RT. Where does one stand: a biological account of preferred interpersonal distance. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2016;11:317–326. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv115.
    1. Holt DJ, et al. Neural correlates of personal space intrusion. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:4123–4134. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0686-13.2014.
    1. Holt DJ, et al. Abnormalities in personal space and parietal–frontal function in schizophrenia. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;9:233–243. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.07.008.
    1. Jacobs, J. M., Cohen, A., Hammerman‐Rozenberg, R. & Stessman, J. Global sleep satisfaction of older people: the Jerusalem Cohort Study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc.54, 325–329 (2006).
    1. Mahon NE. Loneliness and sleep during adolescence. Percept. Mot. Skills. 1994;78:227–231. doi: 10.2466/pms.1994.78.1.227.
    1. Hawkley, L. C., Preacher, K. J. & Cacioppo, J. T. Loneliness impairs daytime functioning but not sleep duration. Health Psychol. 29, 124–129 (2010).
    1. Friedman EM, et al. Social relationships, sleep quality, and interleukin-6 in aging women. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:18757–18762. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509281102.
    1. Kurina LM, et al. Loneliness is associated with sleep fragmentation in a communal society. Sleep. 2011;34:1519–1526. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1390.
    1. Scheele D, et al. Oxytocin modulates social distance between males and females. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:16074–16079. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2755-12.2012.
    1. Bremmer F, et al. Polymodal motion processing in posterior parietal and premotor cortex: a human fMRI study strongly implies equivalencies between humans and monkeys. Neuron. 2001;29:287–296. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00198-2.
    1. Van Overwalle F, Baetens K. Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 2009;48:564–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009.
    1. Saxe R, Carey S, Kanwisher N. Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2004;55:87–124. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142044.
    1. Frith CD, Frith U. Interacting minds--a biological basis. Science. 1999;286:1692–1695. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5445.1692.
    1. Brozzoli C, Gentile G, Bergouignan L, Ehrsson HH. A shared representation of the space near oneself and others in the human premotor cortex. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:1764–1768. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.004.
    1. Gobbini MI, Koralek AC, Bryan RE, Montgomery KJ, Haxby JV. Two takes on the social brain: a comparison of theory of mind tasks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007;19:1803–1814. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1803.
    1. Cacioppo JT, Norris CJ, Decety J, Monteleone G, Nusbaum H. In the eye of the beholder: individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009;21:83–92. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21007.
    1. Kana RK, et al. Aberrant functioning of the theory-of-mind network in children and adolescents with autism. Mol. Autism. 2015;6:59. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0052-x.
    1. Walter H, et al. Dysfunction of the social brain in schizophrenia is modulated by intention type: an fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2009;4:166–176. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn047.
    1. di Pellegrino G, Làdavas E. Peripersonal space in the brain. Neuropsychologia. 2015;66:126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.011.
    1. Candini M, et al. Personal space regulation in childhood autism: effects of social interaction and person’s perspective. Autism Res. 2017;10:144–154. doi: 10.1002/aur.1637.
    1. Cacioppo JT, et al. Loneliness within a nomological net: an evolutionary perspective. J. Res. Pers. 2006;40:1054–1085. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.007.
    1. Adam EK, Hawkley LC, Kudielka BM, Cacioppo JT. Day-to-day dynamics of experience–cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:17058–17063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103.
    1. Cacioppo JT, Fowler JH, Christakis NA. Alone in the crowd: The structure and spread of loneliness in a large social network. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2009;97:977. doi: 10.1037/a0016076.
    1. Heed T, Habets B, Sebanz N, Knoblich G. Others’ actions reduce crossmodal integration in peripersonal space. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:1345–1349. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.068.
    1. Teneggi C, Canzoneri E, di Pellegrino G, Serino A. Social modulation of peripersonal space boundaries. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:406–411. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043.
    1. Pinquart, M. & Sörensen, S. Risk Factors for Loneliness in Adulthood and Old Age--A Meta-analysis. In S. P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in psychology research, vol. 19, pp. 111–143. Hauppauge, NY, US: Nova Science Publishers. (2003).
    1. Mander BA, Winer JR, Walker MP. Sleep and human aging. Neuron. 2017;94:19–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.004.
    1. Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Aging and loneliness: downhill quickly? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007;16:187–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00501.x.
    1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Unhealthy sleep-related behaviors--12 States, 2009. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2011;60:233–238.
    1. Perry A, Rubinsten O, Peled L, Shamay-Tsoory SG. Don’t stand so close to me: a behavioral and ERP study of preferred interpersonal distance. Neuroimage. 2013;83:761–769. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.042.
    1. McGlinchey EL, et al. The effect of sleep deprivation on vocal expression of emotion in adolescents and adults. Sleep. 2011;34:1233–1241. doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1246.
    1. Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: results from two population-based studies. Res. Aging. 2004;26:655–672. doi: 10.1177/0164027504268574.
    1. Kushida CA, et al. Comparison of actigraphic, polysomnographic, and subjective assessment of sleep parameters in sleep-disordered patients. Sleep Med. 2001;2:389–396. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00098-8.
    1. Means MK, Edinger JD, Glenn DM, Fins AI. Accuracy of sleep perceptions among insomnia sufferers and normal sleepers. Sleep Med. 2003;4:285–296. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(03)00057-1.
    1. Ohayon M, et al. National Sleep Foundation’s sleep quality recommendations: first report. Sleep Health. 2017;3:6–19. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2016.11.006.
    1. Gabay AS, Radua J, Kempton MJ, Mehta MA. The ultimatum game and the brain: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014;47:549–558. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.014.
    1. Fehr E, Camerer CF. Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007;11:419–427. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.002.
    1. Mazaika PK, Hoeft F, Glover GH, Reiss AL. Methods and software for fMRI analysis of clinical subjects. Neuroimage. 2009;47:S58. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(09)70238-1.
    1. Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage. 2007;37:90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042.
    1. Friston KJ, et al. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 1994;2:189–210. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020402.
    1. van Veluw SJ, Chance SA. Differentiating between self and others: an ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind. Brain Imaging Behav. 2014;8:24–38. doi: 10.1007/s11682-013-9266-8.
    1. Poldrack RA, Mumford JA. Independence in ROI analysis: where is the voodoo? Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2009;4:208–213. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp011.
    1. Lieberman MD, Cunningham WA. Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2009;4:423–428. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp052.
    1. Segrin, C. Interpersonal communication problems associated with depression and loneliness. In P. A. Andersen, L. K. Guerrero, (eds), Handbook of Communication and Emotion, PP. 215–242 (Academic Press, Elsevier, 1996).
    1. Krause AJ, et al. The sleep-deprived human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017;18:404. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.55.
    1. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988;54:1063. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.
    1. Spielberger, C. D. & Gorsuch, R. L. State-trait anxiety inventory (form Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1983).
    1. Thompson ER. Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2007;38:227–242. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297301.
    1. Marteau TM, Bekker H. The development of a six‐item short‐form of the state scale of the Spielberger State—Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 1992;31:301–306. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb00997.x.
    1. Rechtschaffen, A. & Kales, A. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques, and scoring systems for sleep stages of human subjects. Bethesda, MD (1968).
    1. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep. 2004;27:1255–1273. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.7.1255.

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit