Endogenous opioid antagonism in physiological experimental pain models: a systematic review

Mads U Werner, Manuel P Pereira, Lars Peter H Andersen, Jørgen B Dahl, Mads U Werner, Manuel P Pereira, Lars Peter H Andersen, Jørgen B Dahl

Abstract

Opioid antagonists are pharmacological tools applied as an indirect measure to detect activation of the endogenous opioid system (EOS) in experimental pain models. The objective of this systematic review was to examine the effect of mu-opioid-receptor (MOR) antagonists in placebo-controlled, double-blind studies using 'inhibitory' or 'sensitizing', physiological test paradigms in healthy human subjects. The databases PubMed and Embase were searched according to predefined criteria. Out of a total of 2,142 records, 63 studies (1,477 subjects [male/female ratio = 1.5]) were considered relevant. Twenty-five studies utilized 'inhibitory' test paradigms (ITP) and 38 studies utilized 'sensitizing' test paradigms (STP). The ITP-studies were characterized as conditioning modulation models (22 studies) and repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation models (rTMS; 3 studies), and, the STP-studies as secondary hyperalgesia models (6 studies), 'pain' models (25 studies), summation models (2 studies), nociceptive reflex models (3 studies) and miscellaneous models (2 studies). A consistent reversal of analgesia by a MOR-antagonist was demonstrated in 10 of the 25 ITP-studies, including stress-induced analgesia and rTMS. In the remaining 14 conditioning modulation studies either absence of effects or ambiguous effects by MOR-antagonists, were observed. In the STP-studies, no effect of the opioid-blockade could be demonstrated in 5 out of 6 secondary hyperalgesia studies. The direction of MOR-antagonist dependent effects upon pain ratings, threshold assessments and somatosensory evoked potentials (SSEP), did not appear consistent in 28 out of 32 'pain' model studies. In conclusion, only in 2 experimental human pain models, i.e., stress-induced analgesia and rTMS, administration of MOR-antagonist demonstrated a consistent effect, presumably mediated by an EOS-dependent mechanisms of analgesia and hyperalgesia.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Fig 1. The search algorithm according to…
Fig 1. The search algorithm according to the PRISMA-requirements [126].
Fig 2. Schematic illustration of the ʻinhibitoryʼ…
Fig 2. Schematic illustration of the ʻinhibitoryʼ test paradigms (ITP, upper panel) and the ʻsensitizingʼ test paradigms (STP, lower panel).
The ITP-studies employed an inhibitory conditioning stimulus with evaluation of the associated change in the applied test-stimulus (△test-stimulus). The objective of the ITP-studies was to examine the effect of mu-opioid-receptor (MOR) antagonist on the magnitude of the △test-stimulus, indicating an activation of the endogenous opioid system (EOS) responsible for the conditioning response leading to antinociception/hypoalgesia (the central rectangle [Opioid-dependent mechanism?] indicates a hypothetical augmentation of the conditioning response by the EOS). The STP-studies (lower panel) employed a pain stimulus leading to quantifiable ʻsensitizingʼ CNS-responses, e.g., changes in behavioral measures (hyperalgesia, pain ratings, thresholds, tolerance), nociceptive reflexes, neuroimaging or neuroendocrine variables. In a number of studies a sensitizing conditioning stimulus was applied, e.g., a burn injury [31] and application of capsaicin [35,36], enhancing the nociceptive responses. The objective of the STP-studies was to examine the effect of MOR-antagonist on the magnitude of elicited responses, indirectly either supporting or contradicting an effect mediated by the EOS (the central rectangle [Opioid-dependent mechanism?] indicates a hypothetical attenuation of the response by the EOS). FM Peripheral Conditioning = non-noxious Frequency Modulated Peripheral Conditioning; rTMS = repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation.

References

    1. Willer JC, Albe-Fessard D. Electrophysiological evidence for a release of endogenous opiates in stress-induced 'analgesia' in man. Brain Res. 1980; 198: 419–426.
    1. Willer JC, Dehen H, Cambier J. Stress-induced analgesia in humans: endogenous opioids and naloxone-reversible depression of pain reflexes. Science. 1981; 212: 689–691.
    1. Pertovaara A, Kemppainen P. The influence of naloxone on dental pain threshold elevation produced by peripheral conditioning stimulation at high frequency. Brain Res. 1981; 215: 426–429.
    1. Pertovaara A, Kemppainen P, Johansson G, Karonen SL. Ischemic pain nonsegmentally produces a predominant reduction of pain and thermal sensitivity in man: a selective role for endogenous opioids. Brain Res. 1982; 251: 83–92.
    1. Willer JC, Roby A, Boulu P, Albe-Fessard D. Depressive effect of high frequency peripheral conditioning stimulation upon the nociceptive component of the human blink reflex. Lack of naloxone effect. Brain Res. 1982; 239: 322–326.
    1. Pertovaara A, Kemppainen P, Johansson G, Karonen SL. Dental analgesia produced by non-painful low-frequency stimulation is not influenced by stress or reversed by naloxone. Pain. 1982; 13: 379–384.
    1. Buchsbaum MS, Davis GC, Naber D, Pickar D. Pain enhances naloxone-induced hyperalgesia in humans as assessed by somatosensory evoked potentials. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1983; 79: 99–103.
    1. Jungkunz G, Engel RR, King UG, Kuss HJ. Endogenous opiates increase pain tolerance after stress in humans. Psychiatry Res. 1983; 8: 13–18.
    1. Janal MN, Colt EW, Clark WC, Glusman M. Pain sensitivity, mood and plasma endocrine levels in man following long-distance running: effects of naloxone. Pain. 1984; 19: 13–25.
    1. Willer JC, Ernst M. Diazepam reduces stress-induced analgesia in humans. Brain Res. 1986; 362: 398–402.
    1. Ernst M, Lee MH, Dworkin B, Zaretsky HH. Pain perception decrement produced through repeated stimulation. Pain. 1986; 26: 221–231.
    1. Willer JC, Ernst M. Somatovegetative changes in stress-induced analgesia in man: an electrophysiological and pharmacological study. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1986; 467: 256–272.
    1. Olausson B, Eriksson E, Ellmarker L, Rydenhag B, Shyu BC, Andersson SA. Effects of naloxone on dental pain threshold following muscle exercise and low frequency transcutaneous nerve stimulation: a comparative study in man. Acta Physiol Scand. 1986; 126: 299–305.
    1. Willer JC, Le Bars D, De Broucker T. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in man: involvement of an opioidergic link. Eur J Pharmacol. 1990; 182: 347–355.
    1. Poulsen L, Arendt-Nielsen L, Brøsen K, Gram LF, Sindrup SH. Different effect of naloxone on pain perception in extensive and poor metabolizers of sparteine? Analgesia. 1996; 2: 255–258.
    1. Edwards RR, Ness TJ, Fillingim RB. Endogenous opioids, blood pressure, and diffuse noxious inhibitory controls: a preliminary study. Percept Mot Skills. 2004; 99: 679–687.
    1. Julien N, Marchand S. Endogenous pain inhibitory systems activated by spatial summation are opioid-mediated. Neurosci Lett. 2006; 401: 256–260.
    1. Robertson LJ, Hammond GR, Drummond PD. The effect of subcutaneous naloxone on experimentally induced pain. J Pain. 2008; 9: 79–87.
    1. Rennefeld C, Wiech K, Schoell ED, Lorenz J, Bingel U. Habituation to pain: further support for a central component. Pain. 2010; 148: 503–508. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.12.014
    1. Leonard G, Goffaux P, Marchand S. Deciphering the role of endogenous opioids in high-frequency TENS using low and high doses of naloxone. Pain. 2010; 151: 215–219. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.07.012
    1. Sprenger C, Bingel U, Buchel C. Treating pain with pain: supraspinal mechanisms of endogenous analgesia elicited by heterotopic noxious conditioning stimulation. Pain. 2011; 152: 428–439. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.11.018
    1. King CD, Goodin B, Kindler LL, Caudle RM, Edwards RR, Gravenstein N et al. Reduction of conditioned pain modulation in humans by naltrexone: an exploratory study of the effects of pain catastrophizing. J Behav Med. 2013; 36: 315–327. 10.1007/s10865-012-9424-2
    1. de Andrade DC, Mhalla A, Adam F, Texeira MJ, Bouhassira D. Neuropharmacological basis of rTMS-induced analgesia: the role of endogenous opioids. Pain. 2011; 152: 320–326. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.10.032
    1. Taylor JJ, Borckardt JJ, George MS. Endogenous opioids mediate left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex rTMS-induced analgesia. Pain. 2012; 153: 1219–1225. 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.030
    1. Taylor JJ, Borckardt JJ, Canterberry M, Li X, Hanlon CA, Brown TR, George MS. Naloxone-reversible modulation of pain circuitry by left prefrontal rTMS. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013; 38: 1189–1197. 10.1038/npp.2013.13
    1. Mikkelsen S, Ilkjaer S, Brennum J, Borgbjerg FM, Dahl JB. The effect of naloxone on ketamine-induced effects on hyperalgesia and ketamine-induced side effects in humans. Anesthesiology. 1999; 90: 1539–1545.
    1. Brennum J, Kaiser F, Dahl JB. Effect of naloxone on primary and secondary hyperalgesia induced by the human burn injury model. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2001; 45: 954–960.
    1. Koppert W, Angst M, Alsheimer M, Sittl R, Albrecht S, Schuttler J et al. Naloxone provokes similar pain facilitation as observed after short-term infusion of remifentanil in humans. Pain. 2003; 106: 91–99.
    1. Koppert W, Filitz J, Troster A, Ihmsen H, Angst M, Flor H et al. Activation of naloxone-sensitive and-insensitive inhibitory systems in a human pain model. J Pain. 2005; 6: 757–764.
    1. Chu LF, Dairmont J, Zamora AK, Young CA, Angst MS. The endogenous opioid system is not involved in modulation of opioid-induced hyperalgesia. J Pain. 2011; 12: 108–115. 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.05.006
    1. Pereira MP, Werner MU, Ringsted TK, Rowbotham MC, Taylor BK, Dahl JB (2013) Does naloxone reinstate secondary hyperalgesia in humans after resolution of a burn injury? A placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized, cross-over study. PLoS One. 2013; 8: e64608 10.1371/journal.pone.0064608 PONE-D-13-08611 [pii].
    1. Benedetti F, Arduino C, Amanzio M. Somatotopic activation of opioid systems by target-directed expectations of analgesia. J Neurosci. 1999; 19: 3639–3648.
    1. Price DD, Staud R, Robinson ME, Mauderli AP, Cannon R, Vierck CJ. Enhanced temporal summation of second pain and its central modulation in fibromyalgia patients. Pain. 2002; 99: 49–59.
    1. Graven-Nielsen T, Gibson SJ, Laursen RJ, Svensson P, Arendt-Nielsen L. Opioid-insensitive hypoalgesia to mechanical stimuli at sites ipsilateral and contralateral to experimental muscle pain in human volunteers. Exp Brain Res. 2002; 146: 213–222.
    1. Drummond PD. The effect of peripheral opioid block and body cooling on sensitivity to heat in capsaicin-treated skin. Anesth Analg. 2000; 90: 923–927.
    1. Anderson WS, Sheth RN, Bencherif B, Frost JJ, Campbell JN. Naloxone increases pain induced by topical capsaicin in healthy human volunteers. Pain. 2002; 99: 207–216.
    1. Grevert P, Goldstein A. Endorphins: naloxone fails to alter experimental pain or mood in humans. Science. 1978: 199: 1093–1095.
    1. McCubbin JA, Bruehl S. Do endogenous opioids mediate the relationship between blood pressure and pain sensitivity in normotensives? Pain. 1994; 57: 63–67.
    1. Stacher G, Abatzi TA, Schulte F, Schneider C, Stacher-Janotta G, Gaupmann G et al. Naloxone does not alter the perception of pain induced by electrical and thermal stimulation of the skin in healthy humans. Pain. 1988; 34: 271–276.
    1. Younger JW, Zautra AJ, Cummins ET. Effects of naltrexone on pain sensitivity and mood in fibromyalgia: no evidence for endogenous opioid pathophysiology. PLoS One. 2009; 4: e5180 10.1371/journal.pone.0005180
    1. Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Chont M. What do plasma beta-endorphin levels reveal about endogenous opioid analgesic function? Eur J Pain. 2012; 16: 370–380. 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2011.00021.x
    1. Bruehl S, Burns JW, Gupta R, Buvanendran A, Chont M, Kinner E et al. Endogenous opioid function mediates the association between laboratory-evoked pain sensitivity and morphine analgesic responses. Pain. 2013; 154: 1856–1864. 10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.002
    1. El-Sobky A, Dostrovsky JO, Wall PD. Lack of effect of naloxone on pain perception in humans. Nature. 1976; 263: 783–784.
    1. Buchsbaum MS, Davis GC, Bunney WE Jr. Naloxone alters pain perception and somatosensory evoked potentials in normal subjects. Nature. 1977; 270: 620–622.
    1. Bromm B, Meier W, Scharein E. Antagonism between tilidine and naloxone on cerebral potentials and pain ratings in man. Eur J Pharmacol. 1983; 87: 431–439.
    1. Grevert P, Goldstein A. Effects of naloxone on experimentally induced ischemic pain and on mood in human subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1977; 74: 1291–1294.
    1. Grevert P, Albert LH, Inturrisi CE, Goldstein A Effects of eight-hour naloxone infusions on human subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 1983; 18: 1375–1392.
    1. Posner J, Burke CA. The effects of naloxone on opiate and placebo analgesia in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1985; 87: 468–472.
    1. Schobel HP, Handwerker HO, Schmieder RE, Heusser K, Dominiak P, Luft FC. Effects of naloxone on hemodynamic and sympathetic nerve responses to pain in normotensive vs. borderline hypertensive men. J Auton Nerv Syst. 1998. 69: 49–55.
    1. Cook DB, O'Connor PJ, Ray CA. Muscle pain perception and sympathetic nerve activity to exercise during opioid modulation. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2000; 279: R1565–R1573.
    1. Lautenbacher S, Pauls AM, Strian F, Pirke KM, Krieg JC. Pain perception in patients with eating disorders. Psychosom Med. 1990; 52: 673–682.
    1. Lautenbacher S, Roscher S, Strian D, Fassbender K, Krumrey K, Krieg JC. Pain perception in depression: relationships to symptomatology and naloxone-sensitive mechanisms. Psychosom Med. 1994; 56: 345–352.
    1. al'Absi M, Wittmers LE, Ellestad D, Nordehn G, Kim SW, Kirschbaum C et al. Sex differences in pain and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical responses to opioid blockade. Psychosom Med. 2004; 66: 198–206.
    1. Borras MC, Becerra L, Ploghaus A, Gostic JM, DaSilva A, Gonzalez RG et al. fMRI measurement of CNS responses to naloxone infusion and subsequent mild noxious thermal stimuli in healthy volunteers. J Neurophysiol. 2004; 91: 2723–2733.
    1. Kern D, Pelle-Lancien E, Luce V, Bouhassira D. Pharmacological dissection of the paradoxical pain induced by a thermal grill. Pain. 2008; 135: 291–299. 10.1016/j.pain.2007.12.001
    1. Kotlyar M, al'Absi M, Brauer LH, Grant JE, Fong E, Kim SW. Naltrexone effect on physiological and subjective response to a cold pressor task. Biol Psychol. 2008; 77: 233–236.
    1. Schoell ED, Bingel U, Eippert F, Yacubian J, Christiansen K, Andresen H et al. The effect of opioid receptor blockade on the neural processing of thermal stimuli. PLoS One. 2010; 5: e12344 10.1371/journal.pone.0012344
    1. Pickering G, Moustafa F, Desbrandes S, Cardot JM, Roux D, Dubray C. Paracetamol and opioid pathways: a pilot randomized clinical trial. Fundam Clin Pharmacol. 2013; 27: 339–345. 10.1111/j.1472-8206.2011.01010.x
    1. Boureau F, Willer JC, Dauthier C. Study of naloxone in normal awake man: effects on spinal reflexes. Neuropharmacology. 1978; 17: 565–568.
    1. France CR, al'Absi M, Ring C, France JL, Brose J, Spaeth D et al. Assessment of opiate modulation of pain and nociceptive responding in young adults with a parental history of hypertension. Biol Psychol. 2005; 70: 168–174.
    1. France CR, al'Absi M, Ring C, France JL, Harju A, Wittmers LE. Nociceptive flexion reflex and pain rating responses during endogenous opiate blockade with naltrexone in healthy young adults. Biol Psychol. 2007; 75: 95–100.
    1. Eissenberg T, Riggins EC III, Harkins SW, Weaver MF. A clinical laboratory model for direct assessment of medication-induced antihyperalgesia and subjective effects: initial validation study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2000; 8: 47–60.
    1. Robertson LJ, Drummond PD, Hammond GR. Naloxone antagonizes the local antihyperalgesic effect of fentanyl in burnt skin of healthy humans. J Pain. 2007; 8: 489–493.
    1. Taylor R Jr., Pergolizzi JV Jr., Porreca F, Raffa RB. Opioid antagonists for pain. Expert Opin Investig Drugs. 2013; 22: 517–525. 10.1517/13543784.2013.778973
    1. Pert A, Yaksh T. Sites of morphine induced analgesia in the primate brain: relation to pain pathways. Brain Res. 1974; 80: 135–140.
    1. Richardson DE. Brain stimulation for pain control. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1976; 23: 304–306.
    1. Adams JE. Naloxone reversal of analgesia produced by brain stimulation in the human. Pain. 1976; 2: 161–166.
    1. Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, Jenkinson C, Reynolds DJ, Gavaghan DJ et al. Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Control Clin Trials. 1996; 17: 1–12.
    1. Yarnitsky D, Arendt-Nielsen L, Bouhassira D, Edwards RR, Fillingim RB, Granot M et al. Recommendations on terminology and practice of psychophysical DNIC testing. Eur J Pain. 2010; 14: 339 10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.02.004
    1. Gracely RH. Studies of pain in human subjects In: MacMahon SB, Koltzenburg M, Tracey I, Turk DC, editors. Wall and Melzack's Textbook of Pain. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Saunders; 2013; pp. 283–300.
    1. Glaser EM, Whittaker GC. Evidence for a non-specific mechanism of habituation. J Physiol. 1953; 122: 43–4P.
    1. Rang HP, Dale MM, Ritter JM, Flower RJ. Analgesic drugs In: Rang HP, Dale MM, Ritter JM, Flower RJ, editors. Rang and Dale´s pharmacology. Churchill Livingstone; 2007; pp. 588–609.
    1. Smith K, Hopp M, Mundin G, Bond S, Bailey P, Woodward J, Bell D. Low absolute bioavailability of oral naloxone in healthy subjects. Int J Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2012; 50: 360–367.
    1. Glass PS, Jhaveri RM, Smith LR. Comparison of potency and duration of action of nalmefene and naloxone. Anesth Analg. 1994; 78: 536–541.
    1. Chamberlain JM, Klein BL. A comprehensive review of naloxone for the emergency physician. Am J Emerg Med. 1994; 12: 650–660.
    1. Watson M, Lucas C, Hoy A, Back I. Principles of drug use in palliative medicine In: Watson M, Lucas C, Hoy A, Back I, editors. Oxford handbook of palliative care. Oxford University Press; 2005; pp. 35–79.
    1. Calabrese EJ. Pain and u-shaped dose responses: occurrence, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Crit Rev Toxicol. 2008. 38: 579–590. 10.1080/10408440802026281
    1. Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL. Naloxone dose dependently produces analgesia and hyperalgesia in postoperative pain. Nature. 1979; 278: 740–741.
    1. Levine JD, Gordon NC. Influence of the method of drug administration on analgesic response. Nature. 1984; 312: 755–756.
    1. Levine JD, Gordon NC. Method of administration determines the effect of naloxone on pain. Brain Res. 1986; 365: 377–378.
    1. Gonzalez JP, Brogden RN. Naltrexone. A review of its pharmacodynamic and pharmacokinetic properties and therapeutic efficacy in the management of opioid dependence. Drugs. 1988; 35: 192–213.
    1. Crabtree BL. Review of naltrexone, a long-acting opiate antagonist. Clin Pharm. 1984; 3: 273–280.
    1. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992; 112: 155–159.
    1. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004; 36: 717–731.
    1. Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Chont M. Interacting effects of trait anger and acute anger arousal on pain: the role of endogenous opioids. Psychosom Med. 2011; 73: 612–619. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318227cb88
    1. Pud D, Granovsky Y, Yarnitsky D. The methodology of experimentally induced diffuse noxious inhibitory control (DNIC)-like effect in humans. Pain. 2009; 144: 16–19. 10.1016/j.pain.2009.02.015
    1. van WG, Veldhuijzen DS. Perspective on diffuse noxious inhibitory controls as a model of endogenous pain modulation in clinical pain syndromes. J Pain. 2010; 11: 408–419. 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.10.009
    1. Yarnitsky D. Conditioned pain modulation (the diffuse noxious inhibitory control-like effect): its relevance for acute and chronic pain states. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol. 2010; 23: 611–615. 10.1097/ACO.0b013e32833c348b
    1. Brock C, Olesen SS, Valeriani M, Arendt-Nielsen L, Drewes AM. Brain activity in rectosigmoid pain: unravelling conditioning pain modulatory pathways. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012; 123: 829–837. 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.07.047
    1. Yarnitsky D, Crispel Y, Eisenberg E, Granovsky Y, Ben-Nun B, Best LA et al. Prediction of chronic post-operative pain: pre-operative DNIC testing identifies patients at risk. Pain. 2008; 138: 22–28.
    1. Pielsticker A, Haag G, Zaudig M, Lautenbacher S. Impairment of pain inhibition in chronic tension-type headache. Pain. 2005; 118: 215–223.
    1. Staud R, Weyl EE, Price DD, Robinson ME. Mechanical and heat hyperalgesia highly predict clinical pain intensity in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain syndromes. J Pain. 2012; 13: 725–735. 10.1016/j.jpain.2012.04.006
    1. Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain. 2011; 152: S2–15. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.09.030
    1. van Wilgen CP, Keizer D. The sensitization model to explain how chronic pain exists without tissue damage. Pain Manag Nurs. 2012; 13: 60–65. 10.1016/j.pmn.2010.03.001
    1. Le Bars D, Villanueva L, Bouhassira D, Willer JC. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC) in animals and in man. Patol Fiziol Eksp Ter. 1992; 55–65. .
    1. Latremoliere A, Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: a generator of pain hypersensitivity by central neural plasticity. J Pain. 2009; 10: 895–926. 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.012
    1. Schweinhardt P, Sauro KM, Bushnell MC. Fibromyalgia: a disorder of the brain? Neuroscientist. 2008; 14: 415–421. 10.1177/1073858407312521
    1. Frost JJ, Douglass KH, Mayberg HS, Dannals RF, Links JM, Wilson AA et al. Multicompartmental analysis of [11C]-carfentanil binding to opiate receptors in humans measured by positron emission tomography. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1989; 9: 398–409.
    1. Butler RK, Finn DP. Stress-induced analgesia. Prog Neurobiol. 2009; 88: 184–202. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.003
    1. Troster A, Ihmsen H, Singler B, Filitz J, Koppert W. Interaction of fentanyl and buprenorphine in an experimental model of pain and central sensitization in human volunteers. Clin J Pain. 2012; 28: 705–711. 10.1097/AJP.0b013e318241d948
    1. Ravn P, Secher EL, Skram U, Therkildsen T, Christrup LL, Werner MU. Morphine- and buprenorphine-induced analgesia and antihyperalgesia in a human inflammatory pain model: a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, five-arm crossover study. J Pain Res. 2013; 6: 23–38. 10.2147/JPR.S36827
    1. Koppert W, Ihmsen H, Korber N, Wehrfritz A, Sittl R, Schmelz M et al. Different profiles of buprenorphine-induced analgesia and antihyperalgesia in a human pain model. Pain. 2005; 118: 15–22.
    1. Nickel FT, Ott S, Mohringer S, Saake M, Dorfler A, Seifert F et al. Brain correlates of short-term habituation to repetitive electrical noxious stimulation. Eur J Pain. 2014; 18: 56–66. 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00339.x
    1. Bingel U, Schoell E, Herken W, Buchel C, May A. Habituation to painful stimulation involves the antinociceptive system. Pain. 2007. 131: 21–30.
    1. Bingel U, Herken W, Teutsch S, May A. Habituation to painful stimulation involves the antinociceptive system—a 1-year follow-up of 10 participants. Pain. 2008; 140: 393–394. 10.1016/j.pain.2008.09.030
    1. Mobascher A, Brinkmeyer J, Warbrick T, Musso F, Schlemper V, Wittsack HJ et al. Brain activation patterns underlying fast habituation to painful laser stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. 2010; 75: 16–24. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.10.008
    1. Le Bars D, Willer JC, De Broucker T. Morphine blocks descending pain inhibitory controls in humans. Pain. 1992; 48: 13–20.
    1. Petersen KL, Rowbotham MC. A new human experimental pain model: the heat/capsaicin sensitization model. Neuroreport. 1999; 10: 1511–1516.
    1. Corder G, Doolen S, Donahue RR, Winter MK, Jutras BL, He Y et al. Constitutive mu-opioid receptor activity leads to long-term endogenous analgesia and dependence. Science. 2013; 341: 1394–1399. 10.1126/science.1239403
    1. Altman DG. Better reporting of randomised controlled trials: the CONSORT statement. BMJ. 1996; 313: 570–571.
    1. Moher D, Schulz KF, Altman DG. The CONSORT statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of parallel-group randomized trials. Ann Intern Med. 2001; 134: 657–662.
    1. Schulz KF, Moher D, Altman DG. CONSORT 2010 comments. Lancet. 2010: 376: 1222–1223. 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61879-X
    1. Chan AW, Tetzlaff JM, Altman DG, Laupacis A, Gotzsche PC, Krleza-Jeric K et al. SPIRIT 2013 statement: defining standard protocol items for clinical trials. Ann Intern Med. 2013; 158: 200–207. 10.7326/0003-4819-158-3-201302050-00583
    1. Kilkenny C, Browne W, Cuthill IC, Emerson M, Altman DG. Animal research: reporting in vivo experiments—the ARRIVE guidelines. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2011; 31: 991–993. 10.1038/jcbfm.2010.220
    1. Vignoletti F, Abrahamsson I. Quality of reporting of experimental research in implant dentistry. Critical aspects in design, outcome assessment and model validation. J Clin Periodontol. 2012; 39 Suppl 12: 6–27. 10.1111/j.1600-051X.2011.01830.x
    1. Noordzij M, Dekker FW, Zoccali C, Jager KJ. Sample size calculations. Nephron Clin Pract. 2011; 118: c319–c323. 10.1159/000322830
    1. Werner MU. Sample size and effect size calculations are necessary in clinical studies in order to avoid false positive and false negative conclusions. Scand J Pain. 2013; 4: 163–164.
    1. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ. How many subjects constitute a study? Neuroimage. 1999; 10: 1–5.
    1. Ioannidis JP. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2005; 2: e124 04-PLME-E-0321R2 [pii]; 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124
    1. Walk D, Sehgal N, Moeller-Bertram T, Edwards RR, Wasan A, Wallace M et al. Quantitative sensory testing and mapping: a review of nonautomated quantitative methods for examination of the patient with neuropathic pain. Clin J Pain. 2009; 25: 632–640. 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3181a68c64
    1. Backonja MM, Walk D, Edwards RR, Sehgal N, Moeller-Bertram T, Wasan A et al. Quantitative sensory testing in measurement of neuropathic pain phenomena and other sensory abnormalities. Clin J Pain. 2009; 25: 641–647. 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3181a68c7e
    1. Rolke R, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle TR, Treede RD, Beyer A et al. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): standardized protocol and reference values. Pain. 2006; 123: 231–243.
    1. Geber C, Klein T, Azad S, Birklein F, Gierthmuhlen J, Huge V et al. Test-retest and interobserver reliability of quantitative sensory testing according to the protocol of the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): a multi-centre study. Pain. 2011; 152: 548–556. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.11.013
    1. Maier C, Baron R, Tolle TR, Binder A, Birbaumer N, Birklein F et al. Quantitative sensory testing in the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): somatosensory abnormalities in 1236 patients with different neuropathic pain syndromes. Pain. 2010; 150: 439–450. 10.1016/j.pain.2010.05.002
    1. Werner MU, Petersen MA, Bischoff JM Test-retest studies in quantitative sensory testing: a critical review. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2013; 57: 957–963. 10.1111/aas.12150
    1. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009; 6: e1000097 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit