Low-level environmental lead exposure and children's intellectual function: an international pooled analysis

Bruce P Lanphear, Richard Hornung, Jane Khoury, Kimberly Yolton, Peter Baghurst, David C Bellinger, Richard L Canfield, Kim N Dietrich, Robert Bornschein, Tom Greene, Stephen J Rothenberg, Herbert L Needleman, Lourdes Schnaas, Gail Wasserman, Joseph Graziano, Russell Roberts, Bruce P Lanphear, Richard Hornung, Jane Khoury, Kimberly Yolton, Peter Baghurst, David C Bellinger, Richard L Canfield, Kim N Dietrich, Robert Bornschein, Tom Greene, Stephen J Rothenberg, Herbert L Needleman, Lourdes Schnaas, Gail Wasserman, Joseph Graziano, Russell Roberts

Abstract

Lead is a confirmed neurotoxin, but questions remain about lead-associated intellectual deficits at blood lead levels < 10 microg/dL and whether lower exposures are, for a given change in exposure, associated with greater deficits. The objective of this study was to examine the association of intelligence test scores and blood lead concentration, especially for children who had maximal measured blood lead levels < 10 microg/dL. We examined data collected from 1,333 children who participated in seven international population-based longitudinal cohort studies, followed from birth or infancy until 5-10 years of age. The full-scale IQ score was the primary outcome measure. The geometric mean blood lead concentration of the children peaked at 17.8 microg/dL and declined to 9.4 microg/dL by 5-7 years of age; 244 (18%) children had a maximal blood lead concentration < 10 microg/dL, and 103 (8%) had a maximal blood lead concentration < 7.5 microg/dL. After adjustment for covariates, we found an inverse relationship between blood lead concentration and IQ score. Using a log-linear model, we found a 6.9 IQ point decrement [95% confidence interval (CI), 4.2-9.4] associated with an increase in concurrent blood lead levels from 2.4 to 30 microg/dL. The estimated IQ point decrements associated with an increase in blood lead from 2.4 to 10 microg/dL, 10 to 20 microg/dL, and 20 to 30 microg/dL were 3.9 (95% CI, 2.4-5.3), 1.9 (95% CI, 1.2-2.6), and 1.1 (95% CI, 0.7-1.5), respectively. For a given increase in blood lead, the lead-associated intellectual decrement for children with a maximal blood lead level < 7.5 microg/dL was significantly greater than that observed for those with a maximal blood lead level > or = 7.5 microg/dL (p = 0.015). We conclude that environmental lead exposure in children who have maximal blood lead levels < 7.5 microg/dL is associated with intellectual deficits.

Figures

Figure 1. Restricted cubic splines and log-linear…
Figure 1. Restricted cubic splines and log-linear model for concurrent blood lead concentration. The dotted lines are the 95% CIs for the restricted cubic splines.
Figure 2. Linear models for each cohort…
Figure 2. Linear models for each cohort study in the pooled analysis, adjusted for maternal IQ, HOME score, maternal education, and birth weight. The figure represents the 5th to 95th percentile of the concurrent blood lead level at the time of IQ testing.
Figure 3. Log-linear model (95% CIs shaded)…
Figure 3. Log-linear model (95% CIs shaded) for concurrent blood lead concentration, adjusted for HOME score, maternal education, maternal IQ, and birth weight. The mean IQ (95% CI) for the intervals 20 μg/dL are shown.
Figure 4. Log-linear model for concurrent blood…
Figure 4. Log-linear model for concurrent blood lead concentration along with linear models for concurrent blood lead levels among children with peak blood lead levels above and below 10 μg/dL.

References

    1. Baghurst PA, McMichael AJ, Wigg NR, Vimpani GV, Robertson EF, Roberts RJ, et al. Environmental exposure to lead and children’s intelligence at the age of seven years. The Port Pirie Cohort Study. N Engl J Med. 1992;327:1279–1284.
    1. Bellinger D, Dietrich KN. Low-level lead exposure and cognitive function in children. Pediatr Ann. 1994;23:600–605.
    1. Bellinger D, Leviton A, Waternaux C, Needleman HL, Rabinowitz M. Longitudinal analyses of prenatal and postnatal lead exposure and early cognitive development. N Engl J Med. 1987;316:1037–1043.
    1. Bellinger DC, Needleman HL. Intellectual impairment and blood lead levels. N Engl J Med. 2003;349:500–502.
    1. Bellinger DC, Stiles KM, Needleman HL. Low-level lead exposure, intelligence and academic achievement: a long-term follow-up study. Pediatrics. 1992;90:855–861.
    1. Belsley DA, Kuh E, Welsch RE.1980. Regression Diagnostics. New York:John Wiley and Sons.
    1. Borja-Aburto VH, Hertz-Picciotto I, Rojas Lopez M, Farias P, Rios C, Blanco J. Blood lead levels measured prospectively and risk of spontaneous abortion. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;150:590–597.
    1. Caldwell BM, Bradley R.1984. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock, AR:University of Arkansas.
    1. Canfield RL, Henderson CR, Cory-Slechta DA, Cox C, Jusko TA, Lanphear BP. Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 micrograms per deciliter. N Engl J Med. 2003;348:1517–1526.
    1. CDC1991. Preventing Lead Poisoning in Young Children: A Statement by the Centers for Disease Control. Atlanta, GA:Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
    1. Clark CS, Bornschein RL, Succop P, Que Hee SS, Hammond PB, Peace B. Condition and type of housing as an indicator of potential environmental lead exposure and pediatric blood lead levels. Environ Res. 1985;38:46–53.
    1. Cooney GH, Bell A, McBride W, Carter C. Low-level exposures to lead: the Sydney lead study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1989;31:640–649.
    1. Cory-Slechta DA. Relationships between Pb-induced changes in neurotransmitter system function and behavioral toxicity. Neurotoxicology. 1997;18:673–688.
    1. Dietrich KN, Berger OG, Succop PA, Hammond PB, Bornschein RL. The developmental consequences of low to moderate prenatal and postnatal lead exposure: intellectual attainment in the Cincinnati Lead Study Cohort following school entry. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1993;15:37–44.
    1. Dietrich KN, Ris MD, Succop PA, Berger OG, Bornschein RL. Early exposure to lead and juvenile delinquency. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2001;23:511–518.
    1. Efron B, Tibshirani RJ.1993. An Introduction to the Bootstrap. New York:Chapman & Hall.
    1. Ernhart CB, Morrow-Tlucak M, Wolf AW, Super D, Drotar D. Low level lead exposure in the prenatal and early preschool periods: intelligence prior to school entry. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1989;11:161–170.
    1. Factor-Litvak P, Wasserman G, Kline JK, Graziano J. The Yugoslavia prospective study of environmental lead exposure. Environ Health Perspect. 1999;107:9–15.
    1. Fulton M, Raab G, Thomson G, Laxen D, Hunter R, Hepburn W. Influence of blood lead on the ability and attainment of children in Edinburgh. Lancet. 1987;1:1221–1226.
    1. Harrell F.2001. Regression Modeling Strategies. New York: Springer Verlag.
    1. Lanphear BP. The paradox of lead poisoning prevention. Science. 1998;281:1617–1618.
    1. Lanphear BP, Dietrich K, Auinger P, Cox C. Cognitive deficits associated with blood lead levels < 10 μg/dl in US children and adolescents. Public Health Rep. 2000;115:521–529.
    1. Lanphear BP, Hornung R, Ho M, Howard CR, Eberly S, Knauf K. Environmental lead exposure during early childhood. J Pediatr. 2002;140:40–47.
    1. Lidsky TI, Schneider JS. Lead neurotoxicity in children: basic mechanisms and clinical correlates. Brain. 2003;126:5–19.
    1. Lin JL, Lin-Tan DT, Hsu KH, Yu CC. Environmental lead exposure and progression of chronic renal diseases in patients without diabetes. N Engl J Med. 2003;348:277–286.
    1. Littell RC, Milliken GA, Stroup WW, Wolfinger RD.1996. 1996 SAS System for Mixed Models. Cary, NC:SAS Institute Inc.
    1. Markovac J, Goldstein GW. Picomolar concentrations of lead stimulate brain protein kinase C. Nature. 1988;334:71–73.
    1. Moss ME, Lanphear BP, Auinger P. Association of dental caries and blood lead levels. JAMA. 1999;281:2294–2298.
    1. Nash D, Magder L, Lustberg M, Sherwin RW, Rubin RJ, Kaufmann RB, et al. Blood lead, blood pressure, and hypertension in perimenopausal and postmenopausal women. JAMA. 2003;289:1523–1532.
    1. National Research Council1993. Measuring Lead Exposure in Infants, Children and Other Sensitive Populations. Washington, DC:National Academy of Sciences.
    1. Needleman HL, Gatsonis CA. Low-level lead exposure and the IQ of children. A meta-analysis of modern studies. JAMA. 1990;263:673–678.
    1. Needleman HL, McFarland C, Ness RB, Fienberg SE, Tobin MJ. Bone lead levels in adjudicated delinquents. A case control study. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24:711–717.
    1. Needleman HL, Schell A, Bellinger D, Leviton A, Allred EN. The long-term effects of exposure to low doses of lead in childhood. An 11-year follow-up report. N Engl J Med. 1990;322:83–88.
    1. Patterson C, Ericson J, Manea-Krichten M, Shirahata H. Natural skeletal levels of lead in Homo sapiens sapiens uncontaminated by technological lead. Sci Total Environ. 1991;107:205–236.
    1. Pocock SJ, Smith M, Baghurst PA. Environmental lead and children’s intelligence: a systematic review of the epidemiological evidence. Br Med J. 1994;309:1189–1197.
    1. Rice DC. Lead-induced changes in learning: evidence for behavioral mechanisms from experimental animal studies. Neurotoxicology. 1993;14:167–178.
    1. Rogan WJ, Ware JH. Exposure to lead in children—how low is low enough? N Engl J Med. 2003;348:1515–1516.
    1. Rosen JF, Mushak P. Primary prevention of childhood lead poisoning—the only solution. N Engl J Med. 2001;344:1470–1471.
    1. Schnaas L, Rothenberg SJ, Perroni E, Martinez S, Hernandez C, Hernandez RM. Temporal pattern in the effect of postnatal blood lead level on intellectual development of young children. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2000;22:805–810.
    1. Schneider JS, Huang FN, Vemuri MC. Effects of low-level lead exposure on cell survival and neurite length in primary mesencephalic cultures. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2003;25:555–559.
    1. Schwartz J. Low-level lead exposure and children’s IQ: a meta-analysis and search for a threshold. Environ Res. 1994;65:42–55.
    1. Schwartz J, Otto D. Lead and minor hearing impairment. Arch Environ Health. 1991;46:300–305.
    1. Tong S, Lu Y. Identification of confounders in the assessment of the relationship between lead exposure and child development. Ann Epidemiol. 2000;11:38–45.
    1. Walkowiak J, Altmann L, Kramer U, Sveinsson K, Turfeld M, Weishoff Houben M, et al. Cognitive and sensorimotor functions in 6-year-old children in relation to lead and mercury levels: adjustment for intelligence and contrast sensitivity in computerized testing. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1998;20:511–521.
    1. Wasserman GA, Liu X, Lolacono NJ, Factor-Litvak P, Kline JK, Popovac D, et al. Lead exposure and intelligence in 7-year-old children: the Yugoslavia Prospective Study. Environ Health Perspect. 1997;105:956–962.
    1. Wechsler D.1967. Manual for Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX:The Psychological Corporation.
    1. Wechsler D.1974. Manual for Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Revised. San Antonio, TX:The Psychological Corporation.
    1. Wechsler D.1981. WISC-R-Español. Escala de intelligencia revisiada para el nivel escolar. Manual. Mexico City:El Manual Moderno, SA.
    1. Wechsler D.1991. Manual for Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX:The Psychological Corporation.
    1. WHO1995. Environmental Health Criteria 165—Inorganic Lead. Geneva:International Programme on Chemical Safety, World Health Organization.
    1. Wynder EL. Invited commentary: studies in mechanism and prevention. Striking a proper balance. Am J Epidemiol. 1994;139:547–549.
    1. Yule W, Lansdown R, Millar IB, Urbanowicz MA. The relationship between blood lead concentrations, intelligence and attainment in a school population: a pilot study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1981;23:567–576.
    1. Ziegler EE, Edwards BB, Jensen Rl, Mahaffey KR, Fomon SJ. Absorption and retention of lead by infants. Pediatr Res. 1978;12:29–34.

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit