Visual feedback improves movement illusions induced by tendon vibration after chronic stroke

Salomé Le Franc, Isabelle Bonan, Mathis Fleury, Simon Butet, Christian Barillot, Anatole Lécuyer, Mélanie Cogné, Salomé Le Franc, Isabelle Bonan, Mathis Fleury, Simon Butet, Christian Barillot, Anatole Lécuyer, Mélanie Cogné

Abstract

Background: Illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration is commonly used in rehabilitation and seems valuable for motor rehabilitation after stroke, by playing a role in cerebral plasticity. The aim was to study if congruent visual cues using Virtual Reality (VR) could enhance the illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration of the wrist among participants with stroke.

Methods: We included 20 chronic stroke participants. They experienced tendon vibration of their wrist (100 Hz, 30 times) inducing illusion of movement. Three VR visual conditions were added to the vibration: a congruent moving virtual hand (Moving condition); a static virtual hand (Static condition); or no virtual hand at all (Hidden condition). The participants evaluated for each visual condition the intensity of the illusory movement using a Likert scale, the sensation of wrist's movement using a degree scale and they answered a questionnaire about their preferred condition.

Results: The Moving condition was significantly superior to the Hidden condition and to the Static condition in terms of illusion of movement (p < 0.001) and the wrist's extension (p < 0.001). There was no significant difference between the Hidden and the Static condition for these 2 criteria. The Moving condition was considered the best one to increase the illusion of movement (in 70% of the participants). Two participants did not feel any illusion of movement.

Conclusions: This study showed the interest of using congruent cues in VR in order to enhance the consistency of the illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration among participants after stroke, regardless of their clinical severity. By stimulating the brain motor areas, this visuo-proprioceptive feedback could be an interesting tool in motor rehabilitation. Record number in Clinical Trials: NCT04130711, registered on October 17th 2019 ( https://ichgcp.net/clinical-trials-registry/NCT04130711?id=NCT04130711&draw=2&rank=1 ).

Keywords: Illusory movement; Rehabilitation; Stroke; Tendon vibration; Virtual reality.

Conflict of interest statement

The Authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

© 2021. The Author(s).

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Illustrations of the equipment (example of the positioning of a left arm with a paresis after right stroke). a, b Installation of the vibrator. The forearm was covered with a cloth. ce Presentation of the 3 virtual visual conditions (respectively Moving, Hidden, Static condition). The arrow is not visible during the experiment. f Protractor to measure the sensation of wrist’s displacement. « -90°» signifies a maximal wrist extension for the left upper limb. The description «values of degree» and « wrist extension, wrist flexion» are not seen by the participant during the trial. g Chronology of the trial
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Pictures of the vibratory device UniVibe™. a Vibration motor. b Vibration device linked to the Arduino® and sound isolated. c Wrist positioning
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Flowchart of the experiment
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Boxplot representing the intensity of illusion of movement. Intensity of illusion of movement experienced in each condition, averaged across all participants (respectively for Moving, Hidden, Static condition). The mean is indicated by dots
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Frequency of sensation of wrist extension. The smoothed histogram depicting the frequency of sensation of wrist movement in each condition averaged across the participants. The zero-degree axis is represented by the vertical line (no illusion). The figure represents the distribution of the values from the protractor, with negative degrees for wrist extension and positive degrees for wrist flexion

References

    1. Lecoffre C, de Peretti C, Gabet A, Grimaud O, Woimant F, Giroud M, et al. National trends in patients hospitalized for stroke and stroke mortality in France, 2008 to 2014. Stroke. 2017;48(11):2939–2945. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.117.017640.
    1. Nakayama H, Jørgensen HS, Raaschou HO, Olsen TS. Recovery of upper extremity function in stroke patients: the Copenhagen Stroke Study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil avr. 1994;75(4):394–398. doi: 10.1016/0003-9993(94)90161-9.
    1. Broussy S, Saillour-Glenisson F, García-Lorenzo B, Rouanet F, Lesaine E, Maugeais M, et al. Sequelae and quality of life in patients living at home 1 year after a stroke managed in stroke units. Front Neurol. 2019;10:907. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00907.
    1. Mortaza N, Abou-Setta AM, Zarychanski R, Loewen H, Rabbani R, Glazebrook CM. Upper limb tendon/muscle vibration in persons with subacute and chronic stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med. 2019;55(5):558–569. doi: 10.23736/S1973-9087.19.05605-3.
    1. Rittweger J. Vibration as an exercise modality: how it may work, and what its potential might be. Eur J Appl Physiol mars. 2010;108(5):877–904. doi: 10.1007/s00421-009-1303-3.
    1. Seo NJ, Lakshminarayanan K, Lauer AW, Ramakrishnan V, Schmit BD, Hanlon CA, et al. Use of imperceptible wrist vibration to modulate sensorimotor cortical activity. Exp Brain Res mars. 2019;237(3):805–816. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-05465-z.
    1. Kenzie JM, Ben-Shabat E, Lamp G, Dukelow SP, Carey LM. Illusory limb movements activate different brain networks than imposed limb movements: an ALE meta-analysis. Brain Imaging Behav août. 2018;12(4):919–930. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9756-1.
    1. Naito E, Ehrsson HH, Geyer S, Zilles K, Roland PE. Illusory arm movements activate cortical motor areas: a positron emission tomography study. J Neurosci. 1999;19(14):6134–6144. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-14-06134.1999.
    1. Goodwin GM, McCloskey DI, Matthews PB. Proprioceptive illusions induced by muscle vibration: contribution by muscle spindles to perception? Science. 1972;175(4028):1382–1384. doi: 10.1126/science.175.4028.1382.
    1. Rinderknecht MD, Kim Y, Santos-Carreras L, Bleuler H, Gassert R. Combined tendon vibration and virtual reality for post-stroke hand rehabilitation. In: 2013 World Haptics Conference (WHC). 2013. p. 277‑82.
    1. Tardy-Gervet MF, Gilhodes JC, Roll JP. Interactions between visual and muscular information in illusions of limb movement. Behav Brain Res mai. 1986;20(2):161–174. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(86)90001-X.
    1. Guerraz M, Provost S, Narison R, Brugnon A, Virolle S, Bresciani J-P. Integration of visual and proprioceptive afferents in kinesthesia. Neuroscience. 2012;25(223):258–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.059.
    1. Blanchard C, Roll R, Roll J-P, Kavounoudias A. Differential contributions of vision, touch and muscle proprioception to the coding of hand movements. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(4):e62475. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062475.
    1. Roll JP, Gilhodes JC, Tardy-Gervet MF. Effects of vision on tonic vibration response of a muscle or its antagonists in normal man (author’s transl) Experientia. 1980;36(1):70–72. doi: 10.1007/BF02003980.
    1. Metral M, Chancel M, Brun C, Luyat M, Kavounoudias A, Guerraz M. Kinaesthetic mirror illusion and spatial congruence. Exp Brain Res mai. 2015;233(5):1463–1470. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4220-1.
    1. Le Franc S, Fleury M, Cogne M, Butet S, Barillot C, Lecuyer A, et al. Influence of virtual reality visual feedback on the illusion of movement induced by tendon vibration of wrist in healthy participants. PLOS ONE. 20 nov 2020;15(11):e0242416.
    1. Gladstone DJ, Danells CJ, Black SE. The fugl-meyer assessment of motor recovery after stroke: a critical review of its measurement properties. Neurorehabil Neural Repair sept. 2002;16(3):232–240. doi: 10.1177/154596802401105171.
    1. Norman G. Likert scales, levels of measurement and the « laws » of statistics. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2010;15(5):625–632. doi: 10.1007/s10459-010-9222-y.
    1. Leonardis D, Frisoli A, Solazzi M, Bergamasco M. Illusory perception of arm movement induced by visuo-proprioceptive sensory stimulation and controlled by motor imagery. In: 2012 IEEE Haptics Symposium (HAPTICS). 2012. p. 421‑4.
    1. Tidoni E, Fusco G, Leonardis D, Frisoli A, Bergamasco M, Aglioti SM. Illusory movements induced by tendon vibration in right- and left-handed people. Exp Brain Res févr. 2015;233(2):375–383. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4121-8.
    1. Lackner JR, Taublieb AB. Influence of vision on vibration-induced illusions of limb movement. Experimental Neurology juill. 1984;85(1):97–106. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(84)90164-X.
    1. Hagura N, Takei T, Hirose S, Aramaki Y, Matsumura M, Sadato N, et al. Activity in the posterior parietal cortex mediates visual dominance over kinesthesia. J Neurosci. 2007;27(26):7047–7053. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0970-07.2007.
    1. Beaulieu L-D, Schneider C, Massé-Alarie H, Ribot-Ciscar E. A new method to elicit and measure movement illusions in stroke by means of muscle tendon vibration: the Standardized Kinesthetic Illusion Procedure (SKIP) Somatosens Mot Res mars. 2020;37(1):28–36. doi: 10.1080/08990220.2020.1713739.
    1. Tardieu G, Tardieu C, Lesparogot A, Roby A, Bret M. Can vibration-induced illusions be used as a muscle perception test for normal and cerebral-palsied children? - PubMed [Internet]. 1984. . Accessed 15 Oct 2020.
    1. Rome S, Grünewald R. Abnormal perception of vibration-induced illusion of movement in dystonia - PubMed [Internet]. 1999. . Accessed 15 Oct 2020.
    1. Beaulieu L-D, Massé-Alarie H, Camiré-Bernier S, Ribot-Ciscar É, Schneider C. After-effects of peripheral neurostimulation on brain plasticity and ankle function in chronic stroke: the role of afferents recruited. Neurophysiol Clin sept. 2017;47(4):275–291. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2017.02.003.
    1. Romaiguère P, Anton J-L, Roth M, Casini L, Roll J-P. Motor and parietal cortical areas both underlie kinaesthesia. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res mars. 2003;16(1):74–82. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00221-5.
    1. Mancheva K, Rollnik JD, Wolf W, Dengler R, Kossev A. Vibration-induced kinesthetic illusions and corticospinal excitability changes. J Mot Behav juin. 2017;49(3):299–305. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2016.1204263.
    1. Kim S-Y, Park H, Jung M, Kim KK. Impact of body size match to an avatar on the body ownership illusion and user’s subjective experience. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw avr. 2020;23(4):234–241. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2019.0136.
    1. Seizova-Cajic T, Azzi R. Conflict with vision diminishes proprioceptive adaptation to muscle vibration. Exp Brain Res juin. 2011;211(2):169–175. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2663-6.
    1. Naito E, Roland PE, Grefkes C, Choi HJ, Eickhoff S, Geyer S, et al. Dominance of the right hemisphere and role of area 2 in human kinesthesia. J Neurophysiol févr. 2005;93(2):1020–1034. doi: 10.1152/jn.00637.2004.
    1. Naito E, Nakashima T, Kito T, Aramaki Y, Okada T, Sadato N. Human limb-specific and non-limb-specific brain representations during kinesthetic illusory movements of the upper and lower extremities: Human kinesthesia for the four extremities. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25(11):3476–3487. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05587.x.
    1. Cignetti F, Vaugoyeau M, Nazarian B, Roth M, Anton J-L, Assaiante C. Boosted activation of right inferior frontoparietal network: a basis for illusory movement awareness. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(10):5166–5178. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22541.
    1. Balconi M, Crivelli D, Bove M. ‘Eppur si move’: the association between electrophysiological and psychophysical signatures of perceived movement illusions. J Motor Behav. 2018;50(1):37–50. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2016.1271305.
    1. Goble DJ, Coxon JP, Van Impe A, Geurts M, Doumas M, Wenderoth N, et al. Brain activity during ankle proprioceptive stimulation predicts balance performance in young and older adults. J Neurosci. 2011;31(45):16344–16352. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4159-11.2011.

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit