Effects of acute exposures to carbon dioxide on decision making and cognition in astronaut-like subjects

Robert R Scully, Mathias Basner, Jad Nasrini, Chiu-Wing Lam, Emanuel Hermosillo, Ruben C Gur, Tyler Moore, David J Alexander, Usha Satish, Valerie E Ryder, Robert R Scully, Mathias Basner, Jad Nasrini, Chiu-Wing Lam, Emanuel Hermosillo, Ruben C Gur, Tyler Moore, David J Alexander, Usha Satish, Valerie E Ryder

Abstract

Acute exposure to carbon dioxide (CO2) concentrations below those found on the International Space Station are reported to deteriorate complex decision-making. Effective decision-making is critical to human spaceflight, especially during an emergency response. Therefore, effects of acutely elevated CO2 on decision-making competency and various cognitive domains were assessed in astronaut-like subjects by the Strategic Management Simulation (SMS) and Cognition test batteries. The double-blind cross-over study included 22 participants at the Johnson Space Center randomly assigned to one of four groups. Each group was exposed to a different sequence of four concentrations of CO2 (600, 1200, 2500, 5000 ppm). Subjects performed Cognition before entering the chamber, 15 min and 2.5 h after entering the chamber, and 15 min after exiting the chamber. The SMS was administered 30 min after subjects entered the chamber. There were no clear dose-response patterns for performance on either SMS or Cognition. Performance on most SMS measures and aggregate speed, accuracy, and efficiency scores across Cognition tests were lower at 1200 ppm than at baseline (600 ppm); however, at higher CO2 concentrations performance was similar to or exceeded baseline for most measures. These outcomes, which conflict with those of other studies, likely indicate differing characteristics of the various subject populations and differences in the aggregation of unrecognized stressors, in addition to CO2, are responsible for disparate outcomes among studies. Studies with longer exposure durations are needed to verify that cognitive impairment does not develop over time in crew-like subjects.

Keywords: Psychology; Risk factors.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing interestsThe authors declare no competing interests.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Means ± 95% confidence intervals of SMS measures at each targeted concentration of CO2. The raw scores assigned for each measure are linearly related to performance, with a higher score indicating better performance. Values are based on the relationship to established independent standards of performance among thousands of previous SMS participants. Measures for Initiative are the log-transformed values. *The threshold for significance used for post hoc comparisons by pairwise contrasts of adjusted predictions was p < 0.008, which was derived by dividing 0.05 by 6, the number of post hoc pairwise comparisons made
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Mean ± 95% confidence intervals of percentile ranks for SMS measures at targeted concentrations of CO2. Decision-making performance scores were converted to percentile ranks by indexing against scores of performance measured in more than 20,000 subjects ages 16–83 years who were chosen to represent the working population of the US. The baseline is composed of responses by a variety of members of this population, including students, professionals, homemakers, and laborers
Fig. 3
Fig. 3
Percent change of SMS scores from baseline at elevated concentrations of indoor pollutants determined in several studies. When viewed as a percentage change from the baseline, the SMS measures that were most adversely affected differed among the studies but similarities in the set of most affected measures were greatest between the reports of Satish and Allen. In the Study of Allen most affected measures were the same for CO2 and VOCs. VOCs—volatile organic compounds
Fig. 4
Fig. 4
Mean ± 95% confidence intervals of accuracy (a) and speed (b) for the 10 cognition measures by group at each of the targeted CO2 concentrations (600, 1200, 2500, 5000 ppm). p-Values refer to Type-III fixed effects of variance (with p < 0.05 indicative for at least one concentration differing from the overall mean)
Fig. 5
Fig. 5
Evaluation of standardized scores of speed, accuracy, and efficiency across tests (higher scores reflect better performance). The p-values for significant differences in overall speed across tests achieved at different CO2 concentration are given on the graphs for Overall Speed. Error bars indicate the 95% confidence intervals
Fig. 6
Fig. 6
Sequence and durations of events on days of exposure. The sequences and duration of tests and intervening rest periods on days of exposure are indicated on the time line

References

    1. Manzey D, Lorenz B. Joint NASA-ESA-DARA Study. Part three: effects of chronically elevated CO2 on mental performance during 26 days of confinement. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 1998;69:506–514.
    1. Sayers JA, Smith REA, Holland RL, Keatinge WR. Effects of carbon dioxide on mental performance. J. Appl. Physiol. 1987;63:25–30. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1987.63.1.25.
    1. Selkirk, A., Shykoff, B. & Briggs, J. Cognitive Effects of Hypercapnia on Immersed Working Divers. Report No. NEDU-TR-10-15 (Navy Experimental Diving Unit, Panama City, FL, 2010).
    1. Satish U, et al. Is CO2 an indoor pollutant? Direct effects of low-to-moderate CO2 concentrations on human decision-making performance. Environ. Health Perspect. 2012;120:1671–1677. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1104789.
    1. Allen JG, et al. Associations of cognitive function scores with carbon dioxide, ventilation, and volatile organic compound exposures in office workers: a controlled exposure study of green and conventional office environments. Environ. Health Perspect. 2016;124:805–812. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1510037.
    1. Streufert S, et al. Alcohol and complex functioning. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1993;23:847–866. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01009.x.
    1. Streufert S, Pogash R, Piasecki M. Simulation based assessment of managerial competence: reliability and validity. Pers. Psychol. 1988;41:537–557. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1988.tb00643.x.
    1. Zhang X, Wargocki P, Lian Z. Physiological responses during exposure to carbon dioxide and bioeffluents at levels typically occurring indoors. Indoor Air. 2017;27:65–77. doi: 10.1111/ina.12286.
    1. Zhang X, Wargocki P, Lian Z, Thyregod C. Effects of exposure to carbon dioxide and bioeffluents on perceived air quality, self‐assessed acute health symptoms and cognitive performance. Indoor Air. 2017;27:47–64. doi: 10.1111/ina.12284.
    1. Zhang X, Wargocki P, Lian Z. Human responses to carbon dioxide, a follow-up study at recommended exposure limits in non-industrial environments. Bldg. Environ. 2016;100:162–171. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2016.02.014.
    1. Rodeheffer CD, Chabal S, Clarke JM, Fothergill DM. Acute exposure to low-to-moderate carbon dioxide levels and submariner decision making. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 2018;89:520–525. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.5010.2018.
    1. Stankovic, A., Alexander, D., Oman, C. M. & Schneiderman, S. A Review of Cognitive and Behavioral Effects of Increased Carbon Dioxide Exposure in Humans. NASA/TM-2016-219277 (2016).
    1. Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, R. & Pachur, T. in Heuristics: The Foundations of Adaptive Behavior—Introduction (eds Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, R. & Pachur, T.) (Oxford University Press, Oxford, England, UK, 2011).
    1. Funke J. Dynamic systems as tools for analyzing human judgement. Think. Reason. 2001;7:69–89. doi: 10.1080/13546780042000046.
    1. Johnson MMS. Age differences in decision making: a process methodology for examining strategic information processing. J. Gerontol. 1990;45:75–78. doi: 10.1093/geronj/45.2.P75.
    1. Orasanu J. Crew collaboration in space: a naturalistic decision-making perspective. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2005;76:B154–B163.
    1. Reiskamp, J. & Otto, P. E. in Heuristics The Foundations of Adaptive Behavior. (eds Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, R. & Pachur, T.) Ch. 11 (Oxford University Press, Oxford, England, UK, 2011).
    1. Law J, et al. Relationship between carbon dioxide levels and reported headaches on the International Space Station. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2014;56:477–483. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000158.
    1. Cowings, P. S. et al. Converging Indicators for Assessing Individual Differences in Adaptation to Extreme Environments: Preliminary Report. NASA/TM-2006-213491. (2006).
    1. Seaton, K. A., Kane, R. L. & Sipes, W. Cognitive Assessment during Long-duration Space Flight. NASA/TM-2010-36584. (2010).
    1. Strangman G, Sipes W, Beven G. Human cognitive performance in spaceflight and analogue environments. Space Environ. Med. 2014;85:1033–1048. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.3961.2014.
    1. De La Torre GG, et al. Future perspectives on space psychology: Recommendations on psychosocial and neurobehavioural aspects of human spaceflight. Acta Astronaut. 2012;81:587–599. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2012.08.013.
    1. Roalf DR, et al. Neuroimaging predictors of cognitive performance across a standardized neurocognitive battery. Neuropsychology. 2014;28:161–176. doi: 10.1037/neu0000011.
    1. Basner M, et al. Development and validation of the Cognition test battery. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 2015;86:942–952. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4343.2015.
    1. Curran-Everett D. Multiple comparisons: philosophies and illustrations. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2000;279:R1–R8. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.279.1.R1.
    1. MacNaughton P, et al. The impact of working in a green certified building on cognitive function and health. Bldg. Environ. 2017;114:178–186. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2016.11.041.
    1. Jones, J. M. In U.S., 40% get less than recommended amount of sleep. Well Being. (2013).
    1. Maddalena R, et al. Effects of ventilation rate per person and per floor area on perceived air quality, sick building syndrome symptoms, and decision‐making. Indoor Air. 2015;25:362–370. doi: 10.1111/ina.12149.
    1. Maula H, Hongisto V, Naatula V, Haapakangas A, Koskela H. The effect of low ventilation rate with elevated bioeffluent concentration on work performance, perceived indoor air quality, and health symptoms. Indoor Air. 2016;27:1141–1153. doi: 10.1111/ina.12387.
    1. Guyenet PG, Stornetta RL, Bayliss DA. Central respiratory chemoreception. J. Comp. Neurol. 2010;518:3883–3906. doi: 10.1002/cne.22435.
    1. Guyenet PG, Bayliss DA. Neural control of breathing and CO2 homeostasis. Neuron. 2015;87:946–961. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.001.
    1. Langhorst P, Schulz B, Schulz G, Lambertz M. Reticular formation of the lower brainstem. A common system for cardiorespiratory and somatomotor functions: discharge patterns of neighboring neurons influenced by cardiovascular and respiratory afferents. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 1983;9:411–432. doi: 10.1016/0165-1838(83)90005-X.
    1. Brian JE., Jr. Carbon dioxide and the cerebral circulation. Anesthesiology. 1998;88:1365–1386. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199805000-00029.
    1. Basner M, Mollicone D, Dinges DF. Validity and sensitivity of a brief psychomotor vigilance test (PVT-B) to total and partial sleep deprivation. Acta Astronaut. 2011;69:949–959. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2011.07.015.
    1. Basner M, et al. Effects of −12° head-down tilt with and without elevated levels of CO2 on cognitive performance: the SPACECOT study. J. Appl. Physiol. 2017;124:750–760. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00855.2017.
    1. Maier KL, et al. Protocol for the Reconstructing Consciousness and Cognition (ReCCognition) study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017;11:284. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00284.
    1. Poulin V, Korner-Bitensky N, Dawson DR. Stroke-specific executive function assessment: a literature review of performance-based tools. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 2013;60:3–19. doi: 10.1111/1440-1630.12024.
    1. Satish U, Streufert S, Elsinger PJ. Measuring executive function deficits following head injury: an application of SMS simulation technology. Psychol. Rec. 2006;56:181–190. doi: 10.1007/BF03395543.
    1. Krishnamurthy S, et al. Components of critical decision making and ABSITE assessment: toward a more comprehensive evaluation. J. Grad. Med. Educ. 2009;1:273–277. doi: 10.4300/JGME-D-09-00034.1.
    1. Satish U, Streufert S. Value of a cognitive simulation in medicine: towards optimizing decision making performance of healthcare personnel. Qual. Saf. Health Care. 2002;11:163–167. doi: 10.1136/qhc.11.2.163.
    1. Satish, U. et al. Novel assessment of psychiatry residents: SMS simulations. ACGME Bull. January, 18–23 (2009).
    1. Streufert, S., Pogash, R. M., Piasecki, M. T., Repman, M. A. & Swezey, R. W. Data Collection via a Quasi-experimental Simulation. III: Factor Structure and Validity. Report to the U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences. (1986).
    1. Swezey RW, Streufert S, Satish U, Siem FM. Preliminary development of a computer-based team performance assessment simulation. Int. J. Cogn. Ergon. 1998;2:163–179.
    1. Moore TM, et al. Validation of the cognition test battery for spaceflight in a sample of highly educated adults. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 2017;88:937–946. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4801.2017.
    1. Moore Tyler M., Gur Ruben C., Thomas Michael L., Brown Gregory G., Nock Matthew K., Savitt Adam P., Keilp John G., Heeringa Steven, Ursano Robert J., Stein Murray B. Development, Administration, and Structural Validity of a Brief, Computerized Neurocognitive Battery: Results From the Army Study to Assess Risk and Resilience in Servicemembers. Assessment. 2017;26(1):125–143. doi: 10.1177/1073191116689820.
    1. Gur RC, et al. Computerized neurocognitive scanning: II. The profile of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25:777–788. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00279-2.
    1. Gur RC, et al. Computerized neurocognitive scanning: I. Methodology and validation in healthy people. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25:766–776. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00278-0.
    1. Glahn DC, Gur RC, Ragland JD, Censits DM, Gur RE. Reliability, performance characteristics, construct validity, and an initial clinical application of a visual object learning test (VOLT) Neuropsychology. 1997;11:602. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.11.4.602.
    1. Ragland JD. Working memory for complex figures: an fMRI comparison of letter and fractal n-back tasks. Neuropsychology. 2002;16:370–379. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.370.
    1. Glahn DC, Cannon TD, Gur RE, Ragland JD, Gur RC. Working memory constrains abstraction in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2000;47:34–42. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00187-0.
    1. Benton AL, Varney NR, Hamsher KD. Visuospatial judgment. A clinical test. Arch. Neurol. 1978;35:364–367. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500300038006.
    1. Gur RC, et al. A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2002;15:137–143. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(02)00006-7.
    1. Gur RC, et al. A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: standardization and initial construct validation. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2010;187:254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.11.017.
    1. Perfetti B, et al. Differential patterns of cortical activation as a function of fluid reasoning complexity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:497–510. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20519.
    1. Raven J. The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time. Cogn. Psychol. 2000;41:1–48. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735.
    1. Jewett ME, Dijk DJ, Kronauer RE, Dinges DF. Dose–response relationship between sleep duration and human psychomotor vigilance and subjective alertness. Sleep. 1999;22:51–59. doi: 10.1093/sleep/22.2.171.
    1. Joy S, Fein D, Kaplan E. Decoding digit symbol: speed, memory, and visual scanning. Assessment. 2003;10:56–65. doi: 10.1177/0095399702250335.
    1. Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose–response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26:117–126. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117.
    1. Lejuez CW, et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) J. Exp. Psychol. 2002;8:75–84.
    1. Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011;34:581–591. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.581.
    1. Doran SM, Van Dongen HP, Dinges DF. Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. Arch. Ital. Biol. 2001;139:253–267.
    1. Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilent attention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1129:305–322. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002.

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit