Optimizing Light Flash Sequence Duration to Shift Human Circadian Phase

Daniel S Joyce, Manuel Spitschan, Jamie M Zeitzer, Daniel S Joyce, Manuel Spitschan, Jamie M Zeitzer

Abstract

Unlike light input for forming images, non-image-forming retinal pathways are optimized to convey information about the total light environment, integrating this information over time and space. In a variety of species, discontinuous light sequences (flashes) can be effective stimuli, notably impacting circadian entrainment. In this study, we examined the extent to which this temporal integration can occur. A group of healthy, young (n = 20) individuals took part in a series of 16-day protocols in which we examined the impact of different lengths of light flash sequences on circadian timing. We find a significant phase change of -0.70 h in response to flashes that did not differ by duration; a 15-min sequence could engender as much change in circadian timing as 3.5-h sequences. Acute suppression of melatonin was also observed during short (15-min) exposures, but not in exposures over one hour in length. Our data are consistent with the theory that responses to light flashes are mediated by the extrinsic, rod/cone pathway, and saturate the response of this pathway within 15 min. Further excitation leads to no greater change in circadian timing and an inability to acutely suppress melatonin, indicating that this pathway may be in a refractory state following this brief light stimulation.

Keywords: circadian rhythm; dim light melatonin onset; flash; light; melatonin; sleep.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
CONSORT diagram [18] of participant recruitment and allocation. Of the individuals evaluated, 51% did not meet inclusion criteria or declined to take part in the experiment. Of the participants empaneled, 9.1% were excluded due to non-adherence to target at-home sleep/wake times. Twenty-two participants took part in the in-lab component, quasi-randomized into five participants per illuminance level (see Protocol). The data of one participant (5%) was excluded in the 1 h condition due to failure of the experimental device.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Timing of the laboratory protocol. After stabilizing their sleep/wake rhythms for fourteen days, participants attended the laboratory for a controlled study of the effect of stimulus duration on circadian rhythms. Upon admission to the laboratory on Day 15, participants remained in dim light (

Figure 3

Phase shifting in response to…

Figure 3

Phase shifting in response to light flash sequences of different durations. Left panel…

Figure 3
Phase shifting in response to light flash sequences of different durations. Left panel (A): The magnitude of the shift in the timing of the onset (phase) of melatonin (Δϕ) is plotted against the duration of the flash sequence and compared to a historic protocol control (8) in which individuals were awakened into darkness and not given a light stimulus (0 h data). Right panel (B): The magnitude of the change in acute melatonin concentrations between the beginning and the end of the flash stimulus. Individual data (black discs) are displayed along with group average (green diamonds) with SD bars. The open discs indicate the four participants in whom a fixed, rather than dynamic, threshold was used to determine circadian timing.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Phase shifting in response to light flash sequences of different durations. Left panel (A): The magnitude of the shift in the timing of the onset (phase) of melatonin (Δϕ) is plotted against the duration of the flash sequence and compared to a historic protocol control (8) in which individuals were awakened into darkness and not given a light stimulus (0 h data). Right panel (B): The magnitude of the change in acute melatonin concentrations between the beginning and the end of the flash stimulus. Individual data (black discs) are displayed along with group average (green diamonds) with SD bars. The open discs indicate the four participants in whom a fixed, rather than dynamic, threshold was used to determine circadian timing.

References

    1. Gamlin P.D.R., McDougal D.H., Pokorny J., Smith V.C., Yau K.W., Dacey D.M. Human and macaque pupil responses driven by melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells. Vision Res. 2007;47:946–954. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.12.015.
    1. Allen A.E., Storchi R., Martial F.P., Petersen R.S., Montemurro M.A., Brown T.M., Lucas R.J. Melanopsin-driven light adaptation in mouse vision. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:2481–2490. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.015.
    1. Milosavljevic N., Storchi R., Eleftheriou C.G., Colins A., Petersen R.S., Lucas R.J. Photoreceptive retinal ganglion cells control the information rate of the optic nerve. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115:E11817–E11826. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1810701115.
    1. Joyce D.S., Feigl B., Cao D., Zele A.J. Temporal characteristics of melanopsin inputs to the human pupil light reflex. Vision Res. 2015;107:58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.12.001.
    1. Joyce D.S., Feigl B., Zele A.J. Melanopsin-mediated post-illumination pupil response in the peripheral retina. J. Vis. 2016;16:5. doi: 10.1167/16.8.5.
    1. Spitschan M., Lucas R.J., Brown T.M. Chromatic clocks: Color opponency in non-image-forming visual function. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;78:24–33. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.016.
    1. Berson D.M., Dunn F.A., Takao M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science. 2002;295:1070–1073. doi: 10.1126/science.1067262.
    1. Zeitzer J.M., Ruby N.F., Fisicaro R.A., Heller H.C. Response of the human circadian system to millisecond flashes of light. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e22078. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022078.
    1. Zeitzer J.M., Dijk D.-J., Kronauer R.E., Brown E.N., Czeisler C.A. Sensitivity of the human circadian pacemaker to nocturnal light: Melatonin phase resetting and suppression. J. Physiol. 2000;526:695–702. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.00695.x.
    1. Najjar R.P., Zeitzer J.M. Temporal integration of light flashes by the human circadian system. J. Clin. Investig. 2016;126:938–947. doi: 10.1172/JCI82306.
    1. Chang A.-M., Santhi N., St Hilaire M., Gronfier C., Bradstreet D.S., Duffy J.F., Lockley S.W., Kronauer R.E., Czeisler C.A. Human responses to bright light of different durations. J. Physiol. 2012;590:3103–3112. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.226555.
    1. Rahman S.A., Hilaire M.A.S., Chang A.-M., Santhi N., Duffy J.F., Kronauer R.E., Czeisler C.A., Lockley S.W., Klerman E.B. Circadian phase resetting by a single short-duration light exposure. JCI Insight. 2017;2:e89494. doi: 10.1172/jci.insight.89494.
    1. Buysse D.J., Reynolds III C.F., Monk T.H., Berman S.R., Kupfer D.J. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4.
    1. Babor T.F., de la Fuente J.R., Saunders J., Grant M. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for Use in Primary Care. World Health Organization: Department of Mental Health and Substance Dependence; Geneva, Switzerland: 2001.
    1. Radloff L.S. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977;1:385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306.
    1. Adan A., Almirall H. Horne & Östberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire: A reduced scale. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1991;12:241–253. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-W.
    1. Ishihara S. Ishihara’s Tests for Colour Deficiency. Kanehara Trading Inc.; Tokyo, Japan: 2007.
    1. Moher D., Schulz K.F., Altman D. The CONSORT statement: Revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of parallel-group randomized trials. JAMA. 2001;285:1987–1991. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.15.1987.
    1. Carney C.E., Buysse D.J., Ancoli-Israel S., Edinger J.D., Krystal A.D., Lichstein K.L., Morin C.M. The Consensus Sleep Diary: Standardizing prospective sleep self-monitoring. Sleep. 2012;35:287–302. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1642.
    1. Duffy J.F., Dijk D.-J. Getting through to circadian oscillators: Why use constant routines? J. Biol. Rhythms. 2002;17:4–13. doi: 10.1177/074873002129002294.
    1. Mifflin M.D., St Jeor S.T., Hill L.A., Scott B.J., Daugherty S.A., Koh Y.O. A new predictive equation for resting energy expenditure in healthy individuals. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1990;51:241–247. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/51.2.241.
    1. Joyce D.S., Spitschan M., Zeitzer J.M. Duration invariance and intensity dependence of the human circadian system phase shifting response to brief light flashes. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2022;289:20211943. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2021.1943.
    1. St Hilaire M.A., Gooley J.J., Khalsa S.B.S., Kronauer R.E., Czeisler C.A., Lockley S.W. Human phase response curve to a 1 h pulse of bright white light. J. Physiol. 2012;590:3035–3045. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.227892.
    1. Voultsios A., Kennaway D.J., Dawson D. Salivary Melatonin as a Circadian Phase Marker: Validation and Comparison to Plasma Melatonin. J. Biol. Rhythms. 1997;12:457–466. doi: 10.1177/074873049701200507.
    1. Hoddes E., Zarcone V., Smythe H., Phillips R., Dement W.C. Quantification of sleepiness: A new approach. Psychophysiology. 1973;10:431–437. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1973.tb00801.x.
    1. Dinges D.F., Powell J.W. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 1985;17:652–655. doi: 10.3758/BF03200977.
    1. Gabel V., Kass M., Joyce D.S., Spitschan M., Zeitzer J.M. Auditory psychomotor vigilance testing in older and young adults: A revised threshold setting procedure. Sleep Breath. 2019;23:1021–1025. doi: 10.1007/s11325-019-01859-7.
    1. Basner M., Mollicone D., Dinges D.F. Validity and sensitivity of a brief psychomotor vigilance test (PVT-B) to total and partial sleep deprivation. Acta Astronaut. 2011;69:949–959. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2011.07.015.
    1. Dacey D.M., Liao H.-W., Peterson B.B., Robinson F.R., Smith V.C., Pokorny J., Yau K.-W., Gamlin P.D. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature. 2005;433:749–754. doi: 10.1038/nature03387.
    1. Wong K.Y., Fernandez F.-X. Circadian responses to light-flash exposure: Conceptualization and new data guiding future directions. Front. Neurol. 2021;12:627550. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.627550.
    1. Gooley J.J., Rajaratnam S.M., Brainard G.C., Kronauer R.E., Czeisler C.A., Lockley S.W. Spectral responses of the human circadian system depend on the irradiance and duration of exposure to light. Sci. Transl. Med. 2010;2:31ra33. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3000741.
    1. Ruby N.F., Brennan T.J., Xie X., Cao V., Franken P., Heller H.C., O’Hara B.F. Role of melanopsin in circadian responses to light. Science. 2002;298:2211–2213. doi: 10.1126/science.1076701.
    1. Brown T.M. Melanopic illuminance defines the magnitude of human circadian light responses under a wide range of conditions. J. Pineal Res. 2020;69:e12655. doi: 10.1111/jpi.12655.
    1. Kaladchibachi S., Negelspach D.C., Zeitzer J.M., Fernandez F. Optimization of circadian responses with shorter and shorter millisecond flashes. Biol. Lett. 2019;15:20190371. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2019.0371.
    1. Muindi F., Colas D., Ikeme J., Ruby N.F., Heller H.C. Loss of Melanopsin Photoreception and Antagonism of the Histamine H3 Receptor by Ciproxifan Inhibit Light-Induced Sleep in Mice. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0128175. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128175.
    1. Lucas R.J., Peirson S.N., Berson D.M., Brown T.M., Cooper H.M., Czeisler C.A., Figueiro M.G., Gamlin P.D., Lockley S.W., O’Hagan J.B., et al. Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.10.004.

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit