Effects of Physical-Cognitive Dual Task Training on Executive Function and Gait Performance in Older Adults: A Randomized Controlled Trial

S Falbo, G Condello, L Capranica, R Forte, C Pesce, S Falbo, G Condello, L Capranica, R Forte, C Pesce

Abstract

Physical and cognitive training seem to counteract age-related decline in physical and mental function. Recently, the possibility of integrating cognitive demands into physical training has attracted attention. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effects of twelve weeks of designed physical-cognitive training on executive cognitive function and gait performance in older adults. Thirty-six healthy, active individuals aged 72.30 ± 5.84 years were assigned to two types of physical training with major focus on physical single task (ST) training (n = 16) and physical-cognitive dual task (DT) training (n = 20), respectively. They were tested before and after the intervention for executive function (inhibition, working memory) through Random Number Generation and for gait (walking with/without negotiating hurdles) under both single and dual task (ST, DT) conditions. Gait performance improved in both groups, while inhibitory performance decreased after exercise training with ST focus but tended to increase after training with physical-cognitive DT focus. Changes in inhibition performance were correlated with changes in DT walking performance with group differences as a function of motor task complexity (with/without hurdling). The study supports the effectiveness of group exercise classes for older individuals to improve gait performance, with physical-cognitive DT training selectively counteracting the age-related decline in a core executive function essential for daily living.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
CONSORT flowchart of participants.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Method schematic. Cognitive assessment (RNG) conditions: (1) ST = single task (i.e., RNG only), (2) sDT = simple gait-cognitive dual task (i.e., flat walking + RNG), and (3) cDT = complex dual task (i.e., walking while negotiating hurdles + RNG). Gait assessment conditions: (1) sST = simple single task (i.e., flat walking), (2) cST = complex single task (i.e., walking while negotiating hurdles), and (3) sDT = simple dual task (i.e., flat walking + RNG) and cDT = complex dual task (i.e., walking while negotiating hurdles + RNG). EXP = experimental group; CON = control group.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Training effects on inhibitory performance. Significant decrement (∗∗∗ = p < .001) was seen in the control group (CON), while a marginally significant increment (# = p < .041) was seen in the experimental group (EXP).
Figure 4
Figure 4
Coefficients of variations of gait speed (a) and stride time (b) following the intervention in simple (flat) and complex (hurdling) walking task conditions. ∗∗∗ = p < 0.001.
Figure 5
Figure 5
Correlations between pre-post changes in inhibition (horizontal axis) and coefficients of variations of stride length (vertical axis) in simple (panel (a)) and complex (panel (b)) gait-cognitive task conditions. Regression lines represent significant correlations found for the control group (CON) in the simple condition, but for the experimental group (EXP) in the complex condition.

References

    1. Al-Yahya E., Dawes H., Smith L., Dennis A., Howells K., Cockburn J. Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35(3):715–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008.
    1. Pashler H., Johnston J. C., Ruthruff E. Attention and performance. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:629–651. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.629.
    1. Schaefer S., Schellenbach M., Lindenberger U., Woollacott M. Walking in high-risk settings: do older adults still prioritize gait when distracted by a cognitive task? Experimental Brain Research. 2015;233(1):79–88. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4093-8.
    1. Verghese J., Wang C., Lipton R. B., Holtzer R., Xue X. Quantitative gait dysfunction and risk of cognitive decline and dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 2007;78(9):929–935. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.106914.
    1. Reuter-Lorenz P. A., Festini S. B., Jantz T. K. Executive functions and neurocogntive aging. In: Schaie K. W., Willis S. L., editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 8th. Elsevier; 2016. pp. 245–262.
    1. Vaughan L., Giovanello K. Executive function in daily life: age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. Psychology and Aging. 2010;25(2):343–355. doi: 10.1037/a0017729.
    1. Prakash R. S., Voss M. W., Erickson K. I., Kramer A. F. Physical activity and cognitive vitality. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015;66(1):769–797. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015249.
    1. Young J., Angevaren M., Rusted J., Tabet N. Aerobic exercise to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2015;4 doi: 10.1002/14651858.cd005381.pub4.CD005381
    1. Chang Y.-K., Pan C.-Y., Chen F.-T., Tsai C.-L., Huang C.-C. Effect of resistance-exercise training on cognitive function in healthy older adults: a review. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity. 2012;20(4):497–517. doi: 10.1123/japa.20.4.497.
    1. Voelcker-Rehage C., Niemann C. Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37(9):2268–2295. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.028.
    1. Bherer L. Cognitive plasticity in older adults: effects of cognitive training and physical exercise. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2015;1337(1):1–6. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12682.
    1. Rahe J., Petrelli A., Kaesberg S., Fink G. R., Kessler J., Kalbe E. Effects of cognitive training with additional physical activity compared to pure cognitive training in healthy older adults. Clinical Intervention in Aging. 2015;19(10):297–310.
    1. Schaefer S., Schumacher V. The interplay between cognitive and motor functioning in healthy older adults: findings from dual-task studies and suggestions for intervention. Gerontology. 2011;57(3):239–246. doi: 10.1159/000322197.
    1. Gregory M. A., Gill D. P., Petrella R. J. Brain health and exercise in older adults. Current Sports Medicine Reports. 2013;12(4):256–271. doi: 10.1249/JSR.0b013e31829a74fd.
    1. Wollesen B., Voelcker-Rehage C. Training effects on motor-cognitive dual-task performance in older adults: a systematic review. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity. 2014;11(1):5–24. doi: 10.1007/s11556-013-0122-z.
    1. Eggenberger P., Theill N., Holenstein S., Schumacher V., de Bruin E. D. Multicomponent physical exercise with simultaneous cognitive training to enhance dual-task walking of older adults: a secondary analysis of a 6-month randomized controlled trial with I-year follow-up. Clinical Interventions in Aging. 2015;28(10):1711–1732. doi: 10.2147/cia.s91997.
    1. Pichierri G., Wolf P., Murer K., de Bruin E. D. Cognitive and cognitive-motor interventions affecting physical functioning: a systematic review. BMC Geriatrics. 2011;11, article 29 doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-11-29.
    1. Plummer P., Zukowski L. A., Giuliani C., Hall A. M., Zurakowski D. Effects of physical exercise interventions on gait-related dual-task interference in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Gerontology. 2015;62(1):94–117. doi: 10.1159/000371577.
    1. Forte R., Boreham C. A. G., Costa Leite J., et al. Enhancing cognitive functioning in the elderly: multicomponent vs. resistance training. Clinical Interventions in Aging. 2013;8:19–27. doi: 10.2147/cia.s36514.
    1. Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki A. H., Howerter A., Wager T. D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.
    1. Albinet C., Tomporowski P. D., Beasman K. Aging and concurrent task performance. Cognitive demand and motor control. Educational Gerontology. 2006;32(9):689–706. doi: 10.1080/03601270600835421.
    1. Peters D. M., Fritz S. L., Krotish D. E. Assessing the reliability and validity of a shorter walk test compared with the 10-Meter Walk Test for measurements of gait speed in healthy, older adults. Journal of Geriatric Physical Therapy. 2013;36(1):24–30. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0b013e318248e20d.
    1. Shumway-Cook A., Guralnik J. M., Phillips C. L., et al. Age-associated declines in complex walking task performance: the walking InCHIANTI toolkit. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2007;55(1):58–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00962.x.
    1. Lee M. M., Song C. H., Lee K. J., Jung S. W., Shin D. C., Shin S. H. Concurrent validity and test-retest reliability of the OPTOGait photoelectric cell system for the assessment of spatio-temporal parameters of the gait of young adults. Journal of Physical Therapy Science. 2014;26(1):81–85. doi: 10.1589/jpts.26.81.
    1. Lienhard K., Schneider D., Maffiuletti N. A. Validity of the Optogait photoelectric system for the assessment of spatiotemporal gait parameters. Medical Engineering and Physics. 2013;35(4):500–504. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2012.06.015.
    1. Towse J. N. On random generation and the central executive of working memory. British Journal of Psychology. 1998;89(1):77–101. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1998.tb02674.x.
    1. Audiffren M., Tomporowski P. D., Zagrodnik J. Acute aerobic exercise and information processing: modulation of executive control in a Random Number Generation task. Acta Psychologica. 2009;132(1):85–95. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.06.008.
    1. Priest A. W., Salamon K. B., Hollman J. H. Age-related differences in dual task walking: a cross sectional study. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation. 2008;5, article no. 29 doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-5-29.
    1. Winter D. The Biomechanics and Motor Control Of Human Gait: Normal, Elderly and Pathological. 2nd. Waterloo, Canada: Waterloo Biomechanics; 1991.
    1. Lövdén M., Schaefer S., Pohlmeyer A. E., Lindenberger U. Walking variability and working-memory load in aging: a dual-process account relating cognitive control to motor control performance. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2008;63(3):P121–P128. doi: 10.1093/geronb/63.3.p121.
    1. Plummer-D'Amato P., Brancato B., Dantowitz M., Birken S., Bonke C., Furey E. Effects of gait and cognitive task difficulty on cognitive-motor interference in aging. Journal of Aging Research. 2012;2012:8. doi: 10.1155/2012/583894.583894
    1. Strauss E., Sherman E. M., Spreen O. Trail making test. In: Strauss E., Sherman E. M. S., Spreen O., editors. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Administration, Norms, and Commentary. 3rd. Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 655–677.
    1. Pesce C. Shifting the focus from quantitative to qualitative exercise characteristics in exercise and cognition research. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2012;34(6):766–786. doi: 10.1123/jsep.34.6.766.
    1. Wang X., Pi Y., Chen P., Liu Y., Wang R., Chan C. Cognitive motor interference for preventing falls in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Age and Ageing. 2015;44(2):205–212. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afu175.
    1. Sherrington C., Tiedemann A., Fairhall N., Close J. C. T., Lord S. R. Exercise to prevent falls in older adults: an updated meta-analysis and best practice recommendations. New South Wales Public Health Bulletin. 2011;22(3-4):78–83. doi: 10.1071/nb10056.
    1. Van Abbema R., De Greef M., Crajé C., Krijnen W., Hobbelen H., Van Der Schans C. What type, or combination of exercise can improve preferred gait speed in older adults? A meta-analysis. BMC Geriatrics. 2015;15(1, article 72) doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0061-9.
    1. Hausdorff J. M. Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning. Journal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation. 2005;2, article 19
    1. Gabell A., Nayak U. S. L. The effect of age on variability in gait. Journals of Gerontology. 1984;39(6):662–666.
    1. Newell K. M., Corcos D. M. Issues in variability and motor control. In: Newell K. M., Corcos D. M., editors. Variability and Motor Control. Champaign, Ill, USA: Human Kinetics; 1993. pp. 1–12.
    1. Godde B., Voelcker-Rehage C. More automation and less cognitive control of imagined walking movements in high- versus low-fit older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2010;2, article 139 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00139.
    1. Berchicci M., Lucci G., Pesce C., Spinelli D., Di Russo F. Prefrontal hyperactivity in older people during motor planning. NeuroImage. 2012;62(3):1750–1760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.031.
    1. Hasher L., Zacks R. T., May C. P. Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In: Gopher D., Koriat A., editors. Attention and Performance XVII: Cognitive Regulation of Performance: Interaction of Theory and Application. Cambridge, Mass, USA: The MIT Press; 1999. pp. 653–675.
    1. Von Hippel W. Aging, executive functioning, and social control. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16(5):240–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00512.x.
    1. Shilling V. M., Chetwynd A., Rabbitt P. M. A. Individual inconsistency across measures of inhibition: an investigation of the construct validity of inhibition in older adults. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40(6):605–619. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00157-9.
    1. Noreen S., MacLeod M. D., Kim F. What do we really know about cognitive inhibition? Task demands and inhibitory effects across a range of memory and behavioural tasks. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(8) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0134951.e0134951
    1. Gokalsing E., Robert P. H., Lafont V., et al. Evaluation of the supervisory system in elderly subjects with and without disinhibition. European Psychiatry. 2000;15(7):407–415. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(00)00511-3.
    1. Hortobágyi T., Solnik S., Gruber A., et al. Interaction between age and gait velocity in the amplitude and timing of antagonist muscle coactivation. Gait & Posture. 2009;29(4):558–564. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.12.007.
    1. Hortobágyi T., Finch A., Solnik S., Rider P., De Vita P. Association between muscle activation and metabolic cost of walking in young and old adults. Journals of Gerontology—Series A Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2011;66(5):541–547. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr008.
    1. Ponce-Bravo H., Ponce C., Feriche B., Padial P. Influence of two different exercise programs on physical fitness and cognitive performance in active older adults: functional resistance-band exercises vs. recreational oriented exercises. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine. 2015;24(4):716–722.
    1. Anderson-Hanley C., Nimon J. P., Westen S. C. Cognitive health benefits of strengthening exercise for community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2010;32(9):996–1001. doi: 10.1080/13803391003662702.
    1. Liu-Ambrose T., Nagamatsu L. S., Voss M. W., Khan K. M., Handy T. C. Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33(8):1690–1698. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.05.010.
    1. Liu-Ambrose T., Nagamatsu L. S., Graf P., Beattie B. L., Ashe M. C., Handy T. C. Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2010;170(2):170–178. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.494.
    1. Voelcker-Rehage C., Godde B., Staudinger U. M. Cardiovascular and coordination training differentially improve cognitive performance and neural processing in older adults. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2011;17(5):p. 26.
    1. Liu-Ambrose T., Donaldson M. G., Ahamed Y., et al. Otago home-based strength and balance retraining improves executive functioning in older fallers: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2008;56(10):1821–1830. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01931.x.
    1. Forte R., Boreham C. A. G., De Vito G., Pesce C. Health and quality of life perception in older adults: the joint role of cognitive efficiency and functional mobility. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2015;12(9):11328–11344. doi: 10.3390/ijerph120911328.

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit