Development and validation of the Uniform Data Set (v3.0) executive function composite score (UDS3-EF)

Adam M Staffaroni, Breton M Asken, Kaitlin B Casaletto, Corrina Fonseca, Michelle You, Howard J Rosen, Adam L Boxer, Fanny M Elahi, John Kornak, Dan Mungas, Joel H Kramer, Adam M Staffaroni, Breton M Asken, Kaitlin B Casaletto, Corrina Fonseca, Michelle You, Howard J Rosen, Adam L Boxer, Fanny M Elahi, John Kornak, Dan Mungas, Joel H Kramer

Abstract

Introduction: Cognitive composite scores offer a means of precisely measuring executive functioning (EF).

Methods: We developed the Uniform Data Set v3.0 EF composite score (UDS3-EF) in 3507 controls from the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center dataset using item-response theory and applied nonlinear and linear demographic adjustments. The UDS3-EF was validated with other neuropsychological tests and brain magnetic resonance imaging from independent research cohorts using linear models.

Results: Final model fit was good-to-excellent: comparative fit index = 0.99; root mean squared error of approximation = 0.057. UDS3-EF scores differed across validation cohorts (controls > mild cognitive impairment > Alzheimer's disease-dementia ≈ behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia; P < 0.001). The UDS3-EF correlated most strongly with other EF tests (βs = 0.50 to 0.85, Ps < 0.001) and more with frontal, parietal, and temporal lobe gray matter volumes (βs = 0.18 to 0.33, Ps ≤ 0.004) than occipital gray matter (β = 0.12, P = 0.04). The total sample needed to detect a 40% reduction in UDS3-EF change (n = 286) was ≈40% of the next best measure (F-words; n = 714).

Conclusions: The UDS3-EF is well suited to quantify EF in research and clinical trials and offers psychometric and practical advantages over its component tests.

Keywords: Alzheimer's disease; National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center; cognition; composite score; executive function; item response theory; mild cognitive impairment; uniform data set.

Conflict of interest statement

Dr. Kornak reports providing expert witness testimony for Teva Pharmaceuticals in Forest Laboratories Inc. et al. versus Teva Pharmaceuticals USA, Inc., Case Nos. 1:14‐cv‐00121 and 1:14‐cv‐00686 (D. Del. filed Jan. 31, 2014 and May 30, 2014) regarding the drug memantine; for Apotex/HEC/Ezra in Novartis AG et al. versus Apotex Inc., No. 1:15‐cv‐975 (D. Del. filed Oct. 26, 2015), regarding the drug fingolimod. He has also given testimony on behalf of Puma Biotechnology in Hsingching Hsu et al. versus Puma Biotechnology, INC., et al. 2018 regarding the drug neratinib. Dr. Kramer reports receiving research support from NIH, the Tau Research Consortium, and the Larry L. Hillblom Foundation, and he has provided consultation to Biogen. Dr. Rosen reports having a consulting agreement with Ionis pharmaceuticals. Sources of NIH funding support are listed above. All authors report no conflicts of interest directly related to this project.

© 2020 The Authors. Alzheimer's & Dementia published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Alzheimer's Association.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Standardized factor loadings for the final confirmatory factor analysis model. Trails, Trail Making Test; UDS3, Uniform Data Set version 3.0
FIGURE 2
FIGURE 2
A–B, Scatterplots showing UDS3‐EF scores in the NACC sample as a function of age with separate fit lines for years of education (A) and as a function of education with separate fit lines for age groups (B). NACC, National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center; UDS3, Uniform Data Set version 3.0
FIGURE 3
FIGURE 3
UDS3‐EF score comparison between diagnostic groups. UDS3‐EF score differences among controls, MCI, AD‐dementia, and behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia diagnostic groups in the validation sample. The mean ± standard deviation of the UDS3‐EF score is provided. Box plots represent the median (horizontal line) and interquartile range (top and bottom whiskers) of UDS3‐EF scores. AD, Alzheimer's disease; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; UDS3, Uniform Data Set version 3.0
FIGURE 4
FIGURE 4
Association of the UDS3‐EF score with other cognitive test scores and brain volumes. A, There were 305 participants with a UDS3‐EF and a standard error P < 0.001 for all except Craft Story and Benson Figure). B, Structural neuroimaging data were available for 210 participants (age 66.6 ± 11.4 years old, 52% female, education 16.6 ± 2.5 years; AD‐dementia, N = 52; mild cognitive impairment, N = 46; behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia, N = 37; controls, N = 75). Regression coefficients describe the strength of association between the UDS3‐EF and brain gray matter volumes. Standardized beta‐weights reflect associations after covarying for age, sex, total intracranial volume, and CDR Sum of Boxes (P < 0.005 for all listed βs > .18). AD‐dementia, Alzheimer's disease dementia; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; CDR, Clinical Dementia Rating; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; UDS3, Uniform Data Set version 3.0

References

    1. Packwood S, Hodgetts HM, Tremblay S. A multiperspective approach to the conceptualization of executive functions. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2011;33(4):456‐470.
    1. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. 2000;41(1):49‐100.
    1. Puente AN, Lindbergh CA, Miller LS. The relationship between cognitive reserve and functional ability is mediated by executive functioning in older adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2015;29(1):67‐81.
    1. Cahn‐Weiner DA, Boyle PA, Malloy PF. Tests of executive function predict instrumental activities of daily living in community‐dwelling older individuals. Appl Neuropsychol. 2002;9(3):187‐191.
    1. Bettcher BM, Mungas D, Patel N, et al. Neuroanatomical substrates of executive functions: beyond prefrontal structures. Neuropsychologia. 2016;85:100‐109.
    1. Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta‐analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16(1):17‐42.
    1. Kramer JH, Mungas D, Possin KL, et al. NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):11‐19.
    1. Niendam TA, Laird AR, Ray KL, Dean YM, Glahn DC, Carter CS. Meta‐analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2012;12(2):241‐268.
    1. Reineberg AE, Banich MT. Functional connectivity at rest is sensitive to individual differences in executive function: a network analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(8):2959‐2975.
    1. Rabinovici GD, Stephens ML, Possin KL. Executive dysfunction. CONTINUUM: Lifelong Learning in Neurology. 2015;21. (3 Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry):646.
    1. Swanberg MM, Tractenberg RE, Mohs R, Thal LJ, Cummings JL. Executive dysfunction in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2004;61(4):556‐560.
    1. Chong JSX, Liu S, Loke YM, et al. Influence of cerebrovascular disease on brain networks in prodromal and clinical Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2017;140(11):3012‐3022.
    1. Levit A, Hachinski V, Whitehead SN. Neurovascular unit dysregulation, white matter disease, and executive dysfunction: the shared triad of vascular cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. GeroScience. 2020;42(2):445‐465.
    1. Weintraub S, Salmon D, Mercaldo N, et al. The Alzheimer's disease centers’ uniform data set (UDS): the neuropsychological test battery. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23(2):91.
    1. Brooks BL, Sherman EMS, Iverson GL, Slick DJ, Strauss E. Psychometric foundations for the interpretation of neuropsychological test results. The little black book of neuropsychology. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 2011:893‐922.
    1. Iverson GL, Brooks BL. Improving accuracy for identifying cognitive impairment. The little black book of neuropsychology. Berlin, Germany: Springer; 2011:923‐950.
    1. Gibbons LE, Carle AC, Mackin RS, et al. A composite score for executive functioning, validated in Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) participants with baseline mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6(4):517‐527.
    1. Crane PK, Carle A, Gibbons LE, et al. Development and assessment of a composite score for memory in the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6(4):502‐516.
    1. Crane PK, Narasimhalu K, Gibbons LE, et al. Composite scores for executive function items: demographic heterogeneity and relationships with quantitative magnetic resonance imaging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2008;14(5):746‐759.
    1. Mungas D, Beckett L, Harvey D, et al. Heterogeneity of cognitive trajectories in diverse older persons. Psychol Aging. 2010;25(3):606.
    1. Staffaroni AM, Bajorek L, Casaletto KB, et al. Assessment of executive function declines in presymptomatic and mildly symptomatic familial frontotemporal dementia: nIH‐EXAMINER as a potential clinical trial endpoint. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16(1):11‐21.
    1. Kornak J, Fields J, Kremers W, et al. Nonlinear Z‐score modeling for improved detection of cognitive abnormality. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;11(C):797‐808.
    1. Weintraub S, Besser L, Dodge HH, et al. Version 3 of the Alzheimer Disease Centers’ neuropsychological test battery in the Uniform Data Set (UDS). Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2018;32(1):10.
    1. Salthouse TA. Relations between cognitive abilities and measures of executive functioning. Neuropsychology. 2005;19(4):532.
    1. Pya N, Wood SN. Shape constrained additive models. Stat Comput. 2015;25(3):543‐559.
    1. Kramer JH, Jurik J, Sha SJ, et al. Distinctive neuropsychological patterns in frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and Alzheimer disease. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2003;16(4):211‐218.
    1. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini‐mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189‐198.
    1. Morris JC. The clinical dementia rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43(11):2412‐2414.
    1. Sakpal T. Sample size estimation in clinical trial. Perspect Clin Res. 2010;1(2):67‐67.
    1. Hu Lt, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):1‐55.
    1. Asken BM, Thomas KR, Lee A, et al. Discrepancy‐Based evidence for loss of thinking abilities (DELTA): development and validation of a novel approach to identifying cognitive changes. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2019;26(5):464‐479.
    1. Koenker R. Regression quantiles. Econometrica. 1978:33‐50.
    1. Sherwood B, AX‐H Z, Weintraub S, Wang L. Using quantile regression to create baseline norms for neuropsychological tests. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;2:12‐18.
    1. Kiselica AM, Webber TA, Benge JF. the uniform dataset 3.0 neuropsychological battery: factor structure, invariance testing, and demographically adjusted factor score calculation. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2020;26(6):576‐586.
    1. Koziol LF, et al. Consensus paper: the cerebellum's role in movement and cognition. Cerebellum. 2014;13(1):151‐177.
    1. Farrar DC, Mian AZ, Budson AE, et al. Retained executive abilities in mild cognitive impairment are associated with increased white matter network connectivity. Eur Radiol. 2018;28(1):340‐347.
    1. Nowrangi MA, Okonkwo O, Lyketsos C, et al. Atlas‐based diffusion tensor imaging correlates of executive function. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;44(2):585‐598.
    1. Possin KL, Moskowitz T, Erlhoff SJ, et al. The brain health assessment for detecting and diagnosing neurocognitive disorders. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018;66(1):150‐156.

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit