Hyperactivity persists in male and female adults with ADHD and remains a highly discriminative feature of the disorder: a case-control study

Martin H Teicher, Ann Polcari, Nikolaos Fourligas, Gordana Vitaliano, Carryl P Navalta, Martin H Teicher, Ann Polcari, Nikolaos Fourligas, Gordana Vitaliano, Carryl P Navalta

Abstract

Background: Symptoms of hyperactivity are believed to fade with age leaving ADHD adults mostly inattentive and impulsive. Our aim was to test this assertion using objective measures of hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention.

Method: Participants were 40 subjects with ADHD (23M/17F; 35±10 yrs) and 60 healthy adults (28M/32F; 29±9 yrs) blindly assessed using Wender-Reimherr interview ratings, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders and DSM-IV criteria. Infrared motion capture systems tracked head and leg movements during performance of a No-4's cognitive control task. Subjects also completed the Conners' CPT-II.

Results: ADHD and controls differed significantly in activity and attention. Effect sizes for activity measures (d' = 0.7-1.6) were, on average, two-fold larger than differences in attention or impulsivity, correlated more strongly with executive function ratings and were more discriminatory (ROC area = 0.83 for activity composite, 0.65 for No-4's distraction composite, 0.63 for Conners' CPT-II confidence index, 0.96 for the combined activity and attention diagnostic index). This finding was true for subjects with the predominantly inattentive subtype as well as subjects with combined or predominantly hyperactive/impulsive subtype. Males and females with ADHD were equally active. The superior accuracy of activity measures was confirmed using Random Forest and predictive modeling techniques.

Conclusions: Objectively measured hyperactivity persists in adults with ADHD and is a more discriminative feature of the disorder than computerized measures of inattention or impulsivity. This finding supports the hypothesis that a deficient ability to sit still remains a defining feature of the disorder in adults when it is measured objectively.

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1
Infrared motion analysis patterns tracing the movement path of markers attached to the head, left and right shins and ankles of four representative subjects in 5-minute blocks over the course of the 20-minute cognitive control task. Subjects included in the figure were males and females from each group whose activity measures were closest to the ADHD and control group means.
Figure 2
Figure 2
Receiver operating characteristic curves for age and gender covaried composite measures of attention and activity. Conners’ Confidence Index comes from the Conners CPT-II. Attention Severity, Activity Severity and Diagnostic Composites derived from the No-4’s cognitive control task and infrared motion analysis.
Figure 3
Figure 3
Random forest regression analysis showing the relative importance of activity and attention measures in classifying subjects as ADHD or controls. Variables are rank ordered by importance, which was determined in two ways. The left panel indicates importance by how much the permutation (effective elimination) of a given variable decreases the accuracy of the overall fit. The right panel indicates importance by how much the permutation of a variable attenuates the ability of the specific nodes in the random forest to accurately split the sample. Variables that are associated with the greatest decrease in accuracy or Gini coefficient following permutation are the most important.

References

    1. APA APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.
    1. Wender PH. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Adults. New York: Oxfrod University Press; 1995.
    1. Teicher MH. Actigraphy and motion analysis: new tools for psychiatry. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 1995;3(1):18–35. doi: 10.3109/10673229509017161.
    1. Porrino LJ, Rapoport JL, Behar D, Sceery W, Ismond DR, Bunney WE Jr. A naturalistic assessment of the motor activity of hyperactive boys. I. Comparison with normal controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40(6):681–687. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.04390010091012.
    1. Teicher MH, Ito Y, Glod CA, Barber NI. Objective measurement of hyperactivity and attentional problems in ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35(3):334–342. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199603000-00015.
    1. Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A, Glod CA, Maas LC, Renshaw PF. Functional deficits in basal ganglia of children with attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder shown with functional magnetic resonance imaging relaxometry. Nat Med. 2000;6(4):470–473. doi: 10.1038/74737.
    1. Jucaite A, Fernell E, Halldin C, Forssberg H, Farde L. Reduced midbrain dopamine transporter binding in male adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: association between striatal dopamine markers and motor hyperactivity. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(3):229–238. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.009.
    1. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders - clinician version (SCID-CV) Washington, DCWashington, DC : American Psychiatric Press; 1997.
    1. Ward MF, Wender PH, Reimherr FW. The Wender Utah Rating Scale: an aid in the retrospective diagnosis of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 1993;150(6):885–890.
    1. Brown TE. Brown attention deficit disorder scales for adolescents and adults. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1996.
    1. Kellner R. A symptom questionnaire. J Clin Psychiatry. 1987;48(7):268–273.
    1. Banaschewski T, Yordanova J, Kolev V, Heinrich H, Albrecht B, Rothenberger A. Stimulus context and motor preparation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychol. 2008;77(1):53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.09.003.
    1. Teicher MH, Lowen SB, Polcari A, Foley M, McGreenery CE. Novel strategy for the analysis of CPT data provides new insight into the effects of methylphenidate on attentional states in children with ADHD. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2004;14(2):219–232. doi: 10.1089/1044546041648995.
    1. Paulus MP, Geyer MA. The effectiveness of MDMA and other methylenedioxy-substituted pheylalklamines on the structure of rat locomotor activity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1992;7:15–31.
    1. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. .
    1. Hochberg Y, Benjamini Y. More powerful procedures for multiple significance testing. Stat Med. 1990;9(7):811–818. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780090710.
    1. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2. New York: Academic Press; 1988.
    1. Langlotz CP. Fundamental measures of diagnostic examination performance: usefulness for clinical decision making and research. Radiology. 2003;228(1):3–9. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2281011106.
    1. DeLong ER, DeLong DM, Clarke-Pearson DL. Comparing the areas under two or more correlated receiver operating characteristic curves: a nonparametric approach. Biometrics. 1988;44(3):837–845. doi: 10.2307/2531595.
    1. Breiman L. Random Forests. Machine Learning. 2001;45:5–32. doi: 10.1023/A:1010933404324.
    1. Liaw A, Wiener M. Classification and Regression by randomForest. R News. 2002;2/3:18–22.
    1. Strobl C, Boulesteix AL, Zeileis A, Hothorn T. Bias in random forest variable importance measures: illustrations, sources and a solution. BMC Bioinformatics. 2007;8:25. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-8-25.
    1. Kuhn M. Building predictive models in R using the caret package. J Stat Softw. 2008;28(5):1–26.
    1. Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2010;25(3):157–173. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acq014.
    1. Barkley RA. Differential diagnosis of adults with ADHD: the role of executive function and self-regulation. J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(7):e17. doi: 10.4088/JCP.9066tx1c.
    1. Brown TE. ADD/ADHD and Impaired Executive Function in Clinical Practice. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2008;10(5):407–411. doi: 10.1007/s11920-008-0065-7.
    1. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112(1):155–159.
    1. Ohashi K, Vitaliano G, Polcari A, Teicher MH. Unraveling the nature of hyperactivity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(4):388–396. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.28.
    1. Abikoff H, Courtney M, Pelham WE Jr, Koplewicz HS. Teachers' ratings of disruptive behaviors: the influence of halo effects. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1993;21(5):519–533. doi: 10.1007/BF00916317.
    1. Alderson RM, Rapport MD, Kofler MJ. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and behavioral inhibition: a meta-analytic review of the stop-signal paradigm. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007;35(5):745–758. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9131-6.
    1. van Mourik R, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA. The Stroop revisited: a meta-analysis of interference control in AD/HD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(2):150–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00345.x.
    1. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006.
    1. Balint S, Czobor P, Komlosi S, Meszaros A, Simon V, Bitter I. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): gender- and age-related differences in neurocognition. Psychol Med. 2009;39(8):1337–1345. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004236.
    1. Schweiger A, Abramovitch A, Doniger GM, Simon ES. A clinical construct validity study of a novel computerized battery for the diagnosis of ADHD in young adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007;29(1):100–111. doi: 10.1080/13803390500519738.
    1. Andreou P, Neale BM, Chen W, Christiansen H, Gabriels I, Heise A, Meidad S, Muller UC, Uebel H, Banaschewski T. et al.Reaction time performance in ADHD: improvement under fast-incentive condition and familial effects. Psychol Med. 2007;37(12):1703–1715.
    1. Slusarek M, Velling S, Bunk D, Eggers C. Motivational effects on inhibitory control in children with ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(3):355–363. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200103000-00016.
    1. Kohls G, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K. Hyperresponsiveness to social rewards in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Behav Brain Funct. 2009;5:20. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-20.
    1. Liddle EB, Hollis C, Batty MJ, Groom MJ, Totman JJ, Liotti M, Scerif G, Liddle PF. Task-related default mode network modulation and inhibitory control in ADHD: effects of motivation and methylphenidate. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(7):761–771. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02333.x.
    1. Kuntsi J, Wood AC, Van Der Meere J, Asherson P. Why cognitive performance in ADHD may not reveal true potential: findings from a large population-based sample. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(4):570–579. doi: 10.1017/S135561770909081X.
    1. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121(1):65–94.
    1. Aron AR, Poldrack RA. The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1285–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.026.
    1. Casey BJ, Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Marsh WL, Hamburger SD, Schubert AB, Vauss YC, Vaituzis AC, Dickstein DP, Sarfatti SE. et al.Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(3):374–383. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199703000-00016.
    1. Arnsten AF. Fundamentals of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: circuits and pathways. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67(Suppl 8):7–12.
    1. de Zubicaray GI, Andrew C, Zelaya FO, Williams SC, Dumanoir C. Motor response suppression and the prepotent tendency to respond: a parametric fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(9):1280–1291. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00033-6.
    1. Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(10):1051–1062. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01671.x.
    1. Dillon DG, Pizzagalli DA. Inhibition of Action, Thought, and Emotion: A Selective Neurobiological Review. Appl Prev Psychol. 2007;12(3):99–114. doi: 10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.004.
    1. Feenstra MG, Botterblom MH, van Uum JF. Behavioral arousal and increased dopamine efflux after blockade of NMDA-receptors in the prefrontal cortex are dependent on activation of glutamatergic neurotransmission. Neuropharmacology. 2002;42(6):752–763. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(02)00029-1.
    1. Paine TA, Neve RL, Carlezon WA Jr. Attention deficits and hyperactivity following inhibition of cAMP-dependent protein kinase within the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(9):2143–2155. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.40.
    1. Teicher MH, Anderson CM, Polcari A, Renshaw PF. T2-Relaxometry measures of prefrontal cortex, putamen and vermal balance fully account for differences in activity between children with ADHD and healthy controls. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59(8S):111S–112S.
    1. Pangelinan MM, Zhang G, VanMeter JW, Clark JE, Hatfield BD, Haufler AJ. Beyond age and gender: relationships between cortical and subcortical brain volume and cognitive-motor abilities in school-age children. Neuroimage. 2011;54(4):3093–3100. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.021.
    1. Wunderlich K, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. Mapping value based planning and extensively trained choice in the human brain. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(5):786–791. doi: 10.1038/nn.3068.
    1. Badgaiyan RD, Wack D. Evidence of dopaminergic processing of executive inhibition. PLoS One. 2011;6(12):e28075. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028075.
    1. Simard F, Joanette Y, Petrides M, Jubault T, Madjar C, Monchi O. Fronto-striatal contribution to lexical set-shifting. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(5):1084–1093. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq182.
    1. Ystad M, Hodneland E, Adolfsdottir S, Haasz J, Lundervold AJ, Eichele T, Lundervold A. Cortico-striatal connectivity and cognition in normal aging: a combined DTI and resting state fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2011;55(1):24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.016.
    1. Hartberg CB, Sundet K, Rimol LM, Haukvik UK, Lange EH, Nesvag R, Melle I, Andreassen OA, Agartz I. Subcortical brain volumes relate to neurocognition in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder and healthy controls. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35(4):1122–1130. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.03.014.
    1. Brooks DJ. Dopaminergic action beyond its effects on motor function: imaging studies. J Neurol. 2006;253(Suppl 4):IV8–IV15.
    1. Epstein JN, Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Erhardt D. Continuous performance test results of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;12(2):155–168. doi: 10.1076/clin.12.2.155.2000.
    1. Edwards MC, Gardner ES, Chelonis JJ, Schulz EG, Flake RA, Diaz PF. Estimates of the validity and utility of the Conners' Continuous Performance Test in the assessment of inattentive and/or hyperactive-impulsive behaviors in children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2007;35(3):393–404. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9098-3.

Source: PubMed

3
Předplatit