Force-field compensation in a manual tracking task

Valentina Squeri, Lorenzo Masia, Maura Casadio, Pietro Morasso, Elena Vergaro, Valentina Squeri, Lorenzo Masia, Maura Casadio, Pietro Morasso, Elena Vergaro

Abstract

This study addresses force/movement control in a dynamic "hybrid" task: the master sub-task is continuous manual tracking of a target moving along an eight-shaped Lissajous figure, with the tracking error as the primary performance index; the slave sub-task is compensation of a disturbing curl viscous field, compatibly with the primary performance index. The two sub-tasks are correlated because the lateral force the subject must exert on the eight-shape must be proportional to the longitudinal movement speed in order to perform a good tracking. The results confirm that visuo-manual tracking is characterized by an intermittent control mechanism, in agreement with previous work; the novel finding is that the overall control patterns are not altered by the presence of a large deviating force field, if compared with the undisturbed condition. It is also found that the control of interaction-forces is achieved by a combination of arm stiffness properties and direct force control, as suggested by the systematic lateral deviation of the trajectories from the nominal path and the comparison between perturbed trials and catch trials. The coordination of the two sub-tasks is quickly learnt after the activation of the deviating force field and is achieved by a combination of force and the stiffness components (about 80% vs. 20%), which is a function of the implicit accuracy of the tracking task.

Conflict of interest statement

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Figures

Figure 1. Experimental setup.
Figure 1. Experimental setup.
A: Haptic manipulandum BdF used in the experiments. B: Dynamic visual display. The red circle identifies the position of the target and the white circle the position of the hand; both have a 2 cm diameter. The eight-shaped pathway is displayed in the background for one group of subject. For the other group the background is uniformly black. The ideal eight-shaped path is ±15.7 cm wide and ±9 cm high; total length is equal to 102 cm; the nominal circling period is 8 s. C: Real-time flow of information among the four main modules of the robotic manipulandum (BdF haptic manipulandum; Motion analysis and performance evaluation; Task controller; Force-field generator); : position vectors of the target and the hand, respectively; D = 6 cm: threshold for the tracking error e; : target speed modulating function; T = 8s: nominal circling time; b = 100 N/m/s: viscous coefficient of the curl field . D: Profile of the target speed modulating gain.
Figure 2. Characteristics of errorless trajectories.
Figure 2. Characteristics of errorless trajectories.
A: Speed and curvature profiles of the target in the case of ideal, errorless tracking performance. The curvature scale (in m−1) is divided by 200. The correlation coefficient of the two curves is equal to 0.91. B: Pattern of force determined by the curl field, in the case of ideal, errorless tracking performance. The force vectors, perpendicular to the eight-shaped path and directed to the right of the instantaneous velocity vector, range between 8.3N and 18.3N.
Figure 3. Tracking error and lateral deviation.
Figure 3. Tracking error and lateral deviation.
A: Decomposition of the tracking error into a longitudinal and normal component. : position vectors of the target and the hand, respectively; : longitudinal and normal unit-vectors, respectively. B: Characterization of a catch trial in terms of the initial and final values of the lateral deviation (LDini and LDfin, respectively).
Figure 4. Response patterns of one subject…
Figure 4. Response patterns of one subject at the end of the familiarization phase during two consecutive turns (FA2).
A: Trajectories of the subject (red) and trajectory of the target (blue) for the whole phase. B: Time course of the speed (blue for the target and red for the hand), curvature (blue for the target and red for the hand), and tracking error (green).
Figure 5. Learning curve of the tracking…
Figure 5. Learning curve of the tracking error δ for the whole population of subjects: mean ± standard error.
FA: familiarization phase (60 turns); FF: force-field phase (60 turns); WO: wash-out phase (30 turns).
Figure 6. Response patterns at the beginning…
Figure 6. Response patterns at the beginning of the force field phase during two consecutive turns (FF1).
A: Trajectories of the subject (red) and trajectory of the target (blue) for the whole phase. B: Time course of the speed (blue for the target and red for the hand), curvature (blue for the target and red for the hand), and tracking error (green).
Figure 7. Response patterns at the end…
Figure 7. Response patterns at the end of the force field phase during two consecutive turns (FF2).
A: Trajectories of the subject (red) and trajectory of the target (blue) for the whole phase. B: Time course of the speed (blue for the target and red for the hand), curvature (blue for the target and red for the hand), and tracking error (green).
Figure 8. Comparison of the tracking performance…
Figure 8. Comparison of the tracking performance indicators between the end of the familiarization phase (FA2) and the end of the force field phase (FF2).
The data are averaged over the whole population. Each box shows the mean value (central line) and the mean±SE (lower and upper line). The lines extending from each end of the box represent the mean±SD. DUR (duration of each turn); NP (number of peaks for each turn of the hand speed profile); FE (figural error); CC (correlation coefficient between speed and curvature of the hand); (tracking error); (longitudinal component of the tracking error: negative means that the hand is lagging); (lateral or normal component of the tracking error: negative means that the hand is deviated on the right of the target direction). NP, FE, CC and exhibit significant differences between FA2 and FF2 at the p = 0.00000, p = 0.008340, p = 0.000028 and p = 0.000025 significance level, respectively.
Figure 9. Response patterns during rightward catch…
Figure 9. Response patterns during rightward catch trials of FF2 phase.
Figure 10. Block diagram of the hybrid…
Figure 10. Block diagram of the hybrid control task.

References

    1. Ohta K, Svinin M, Luo Z, Hosoe S, Laboissière R. Optimal trajectory formation of constrained human arm movements. Biol Cybern. 2004;91:23–36.
    1. Russell D, Hogan N. Dealing with constraints: A biomechanical approach. 1989. pp. 892–893. In: Proceedings of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biological Society 11th Annual Conference.
    1. Raibert M, Craig J. Hybrid position/force control of manipulators. Trans ASME. 1981;102:126–133.
    1. Hogan N. Impedance control: an approach to manipulation: Part I-Theory, Part II-Implementation, Part III-Applications ASME. J Dynam Syst Meas Contr. 1985;107:1–23.
    1. Favilla M, Hening W, Ghez C. Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. VI. Independent specification of response amplitude and direction. Exp Brain Res. 1989;75:280–294.
    1. Morasso P. Spatial control of arm movements. Exp Brain Res. 1981;42:223–227.
    1. Venkadesan M, Valero-Cuevas FJ. Neural Control of Motion-to-Force Transitions with the Fingertip. J Neurosci. 2008;28:1366–1373.
    1. Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J Neurosci. 1994;14:3208–3224.
    1. Burdet E, Osu R, Franklin DW, Milner TE, Kawato M. The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance. Nature. 2001;414:446–449.
    1. Poulton E. Tracking skill and manual control. 1974. Academic Press, New York.
    1. Steinbach MJ. Eye tracking of self-moved targets: the role of efference. J Exp Psychol. 1969;82:366–376.
    1. Angel RW, Garland H. Transfer of information from manual to oculomotor control system. J Exp Psychol. 1972;96:92–96.
    1. Hayhoe MM, Shrivastava A, Mruczek R, Pelz JB. Visual memory and motor planning in a natural task. J Vis. 2003;3:49–63.
    1. Navas F, Stark L. Sampling or intermittency in hand control system dynamics. Biophys J. 1968;8:252–302.
    1. Miall RC, Weir DJ, Stein JF. Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks. J Mot Behav. 1993;25:53–63.
    1. Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Winter JL, Stein JF. Evidence for an error deadzone in compensatory tracking. J Mot Behav. 1992;24:299–308.
    1. Hanneton S, Berthoz A, Droulez J, Slotine JJ. Does the brain use sliding variables for the control of movements? Biol Cybern. 1997;77:381–393.
    1. Viviani P, Terzuolo C. Trajectory determines movement dynamics. Neuroscience. 1982;7:431–437.
    1. Pasalar S, Roitman AV, Ebner TJ. Effects of speeds and force fields on submovements during circular manual tracking in humans. Exp Brain Res. 2005;163:214–225.
    1. Roitman AV, Massaquoi SG, Takahashi K, Ebner TJ. Kinematic Analysis of Manual Tracking in Monkeys: Characterization of Movement Intermittencies During a Circular Tracking Task. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:901–911.
    1. Casadio M, Sanguineti V, Morasso PG, Arrichiello V. Braccio di Ferro: a new haptic workstation for neuromotor rehabilitation. Technol Health Care. 2006;14:123–142.
    1. Morasso P, Mussa Ivaldi FA. Trajectory formation and handwriting: a computational model. Biol Cybern. 1982;45:131–142.
    1. Tsuji T, Morasso PG, Goto K, Ito K. Human hand impedance characteristics during maintained posture. Biol Cybern. 1995;72:475–485.
    1. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113.
    1. Conditt MA, Mussa-Ivaldi FA. Central representation of time during motor learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96:11625–11630.
    1. Kearney RE, Hunter IW. Dynamics of human ankle stiffness: variation with displacement amplitude. J Biomech. 1982;15:753–756.
    1. Bizzi E, Polit A, Morasso P. Mechanisms underlying recovery of final head position. J Neurophysiol. 1976;39:435–444.
    1. Feldman A. Functional tuning of the nervous system during control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. II. Controllable parameters of the muscles. Biophysics. 1966;11:565–578.
    1. Feldman A. Functional tuning of the nervous system during control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. III. Mechanographic analysis of the execution by man of the simplest motor task. Biophysics. 1966;11:766–775.
    1. McIntyre J, Gurfinkel EV, Lipshits MI, Droulez J, Gurfinkel VS. Measurements of human force control during a constrained arm motion using a force-actuated joystick. J Neurophysiol. 1995;73:1201–1222.
    1. Chib VS, Krutky MA, Lynch KM, Mussa-Ivaldi FA. The Separate Neural Control of Hand Movements and Contact Forces. J Neurosci. 2009;29:3939–3947.
    1. Nisky I, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Karniel A. A Regression and Boundary-Crossing-Based Model for the Perception of Delayed Stiffness. IEEE Transactions on Haptics. 2008;1:73–82.
    1. Mugge W, Schuurmans J, Schouten AC, van der Helm FCT. Sensory Weighting of Force and Position Feedback in Human Motor Control Tasks. J Neurosci. 2009;29:5476–5482.
    1. Mugge W, Abbink DA, Schouten AC, Dewald JP, van der Helm FC. A rigorous model of reflex function indicates that position and force feedback are flexibly tuned to position and force tasks. Exp Brain Res. 2010;200:325–340.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonneren