Assessing and conceptualizing frontal EEG asymmetry: An updated primer on recording, processing, analyzing, and interpreting frontal alpha asymmetry

Ezra E Smith, Samantha J Reznik, Jennifer L Stewart, John J B Allen, Ezra E Smith, Samantha J Reznik, Jennifer L Stewart, John J B Allen

Abstract

Frontal electroencephalographic (EEG) alpha asymmetry is widely researched in studies of emotion, motivation, and psychopathology, yet it is a metric that has been quantified and analyzed using diverse procedures, and diversity in procedures muddles cross-study interpretation. The aim of this article is to provide an updated tutorial for EEG alpha asymmetry recording, processing, analysis, and interpretation, with an eye towards improving consistency of results across studies. First, a brief background in alpha asymmetry findings is provided. Then, some guidelines for recording, processing, and analyzing alpha asymmetry are presented with an emphasis on the creation of asymmetry scores, referencing choices, and artifact removal. Processing steps are explained in detail, and references to MATLAB-based toolboxes that are helpful for creating and investigating alpha asymmetry are noted. Then, conceptual challenges and interpretative issues are reviewed, including a discussion of alpha asymmetry as a mediator/moderator of emotion and psychopathology. Finally, the effects of two automated component-based artifact correction algorithms-MARA and ADJUST-on frontal alpha asymmetry are evaluated.

Keywords: Frontal EEG asymmetry; ICA artifact correction; Signal processing; Statistical models.

Copyright © 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Figures

Figure A1.
Figure A1.
Histograms of percent of variance rejected for ADJUST (top panel), MARA (middle panel), and the overlap between ADJUST and MARA (bottom panel).
Figure A2.
Figure A2.
Power across all scalp sites (top panel), frontal channels only (F1, F2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, and Fz, middle panel), occipital channels only (POz, Oz, O1, and O2, bottom panel) at each spectral point for data that was visually inspected data, visually inspected + ADJUST, and visually inspected + MARA.. All 3 conditions show a robust alpha peak, MARA and ADJUST similarly attenuate delta and theta power, and MARA attenuates high frequency power at frontal channels.
Figure A3.
Figure A3.
Signal-to-noise ratio across all scalp sites (top panel), frontal channels only (F1, F2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, and Fz, middle panel), occipital channels only (POz, Oz, O1, and O2, bottom panel) at each spectral point for data that was visually inspected data, visually inspected + ADJUST, and visually inspected + MARA. MARA and ADJUST increase SNR over frontal channels.
Figure A4.
Figure A4.
Spectral power differences for single recordings (N≥2480) across all scalp sites (top panel), frontal channels only (F1, F2, F3, F4, F5, F6, F7, F8, and Fz, middle panel), occipital channels only (POz, Oz, O1, and O2, bottom panel) at each spectral point for data that was visually inspected data, visually inspected + ADJUST, and visually inspected + MARA. MARA and ADJUST produce generally consistent results across recordings. Both ADJUST and MARA adequately preserved alpha power in most, but not all cases. MARA more frequently reduced high frequency power over frontal channels than ADJUST.
Figure A5.
Figure A5.
Histograms of asymmetry scores for different artifact mitigation approaches at a frequently used asymmetry site (F6/F5). Histograms of asymmetry scores following visual inspection (top panel), visual inspection + ADJUST (top-middle panel), and visual inspection + MARA (bottom-middle panel), and the overlap between three artifact approaches (bottom panel) show that MARA reduces the pointedness of the distribution in asymmetry scores around zero compared to visual inspection only, and visual inspection + ADJUST.
Figure A6.
Figure A6.
Correlations for asymmetry scores between different methods of artifact mitigation at four frontal electrodes. Pearson correlations between 3 datasets are presented: visually inspected data, visually inspected + ADJUST, and visually inspected + MARA cleaned data.. Correlations between visually inspected data and visually inspected + component cleaned data were high in every case. Interestingly, the lowest correlations were between visual inspected data and MARA, and between ADJUST and MARA, at those electrodes that also tended to have the greatest high frequency power, presumably from frontalis activity (i.e., F4/F3 and F6/F5).
Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Topography of alpha power under eyes open (top) and eyes closed (bottom) conditions as a function of transformation (Cz, average (AR), or linked mastoid (LM) reference or current source density (CSD) transformation) from a sample of over 2400 recordings. Power values at each site represent natural-log transformed values; thus negative numbers represent mean power values less than one. Each transformation is scaled independently, but within each transformation, eyes open and closed data are plotted on the same scale. Only the CSD transformation contains occipital alpha to occipital leads, whereas the other three montages show reflected alpha at frontal regions, visible most clearly by a comparison of frontal leads under eyes closed compared to eyes open recordings.
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
Overview of converting time-domain signals to power spectra for EEG asymmetry research. Panel A depicts a 10-second segment of raw data from a single channel on the left, and the spectral representation of this epoch on the right. Panel B illustrates the process of epoching the longer segment into shorter overlapping two-second epochs. Panel C shows the impact of the Hamming window (dotted bell curve) on a single epoch, with the grey line representing the raw signal and the black line representing the signal after the application of the window. Note that a discontinuity would result if a copy of the raw (grey) signal were concatenated following this signal, but no such discontinuity would result for a similarly concatenated windowed (black) signal. Panel D displays the net weighting (black line, scaled to fit graph) of overlapping hamming windows (grey lines) for two-second epochs. Panel E illustrates the impact of averaging power spectra. The top 9 grey lines are the spectral representation of 9 two-second epochs, and the lower black line is the average spectrum. Note that alpha power (8–13 Hz) is somewhat variable from epoch to epoch, but that the average spectrum reveals a distinct alpha peak. Vertical axis in Panel E is power in microvolts-squared. Figure after Allen, Coan, and Nazarian (2004).
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
Spectral power across the scalp for five frequency bands of interest before and after automatic IC-based correction using the ADJUST algorithm. The depicted scale (inμV2) varies across frequency bins, and is the same for the first two columns (Visual inspection only, Visual inspection only + ADJUST; the difference score has its own scale)). Overlapping-epochs were hamming-windowed prior to FFT to mitigate edge-artifacts. The FFT results for each epoch were averaged for each subject, then across all subjects (i.e., a grand-average). Spectral points were averaged within canonical frequency bands.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
Spectral power across the scalp for five frequency bands of interest before and after automatic IC-based correction using the MARA algorithm. The depicted scale (in μV2) varies across frequency bins, and is the same for the first two columns (Visual inspection only, Visual inspection only + MARA; the difference score has its own scale)). Overlapping-epochs were hamming-windowed prior to FFT to mitigate edge-artifacts. The FFT results for each epoch were averaged for each subject, then across all subjects (i.e., a grand-average). Spectral points were averaged within canonical frequency bands.
Figure 5.
Figure 5.
This figure illustrates conceptual differences between the third variables of moderator, mediator, covariate, and confound, adapting diagrams from Baron and Kenny (1986). As described in further detail in the text, a moderator is a third variable that changes the relationship between a predictor and outcome variable. Covariates do not alter but can clarify the relationship between predictor and outcome by adjusting for variance between a third variable and the outcome. On the other hand, mediators are third variables through which a predictor variable changes an outcome variable. Mediators may be distinguished from confounds, which are third variables that share the same statistical relationships but cannot reasonably be the cause by which the predictor affects change.

References

    1. Ahern GL, & Schwartz GE (1985). Differential lateralization for positive and negative emotion in the human brain: EEG spectral analysis. Neuropsychologia, 23, 745–755.
    1. Allen JJ, Coan JA, & Nazarian M (2004). Issues and assumptions on the road from raw signals to metrics of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion. Biological Psychology, 67, 183–218.
    1. Allen JJB, & Cohen MX (2010). Deconstructing the “resting” state: Exploring the temporal dynamics of frontal alpha asymmetry as an endophenotype for depression. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 232 doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00232.
    1. Allen JJB, Hewig JS, Hecht H, Miltner WHR, & Schnyer DM (2013). Linking resting state EEG asymmetry to resting state fMRI with simultaneous recordings. Psychophysiology, 50, S35.
    1. Allen JJB, McKnight KM, Moreno FA, Demaree HA, & Delgado PL (2009). Alteration of frontal EEG asymmetry during tryptophan depletion predicts future depression. Journal of Affective Disorders,115,189–195.
    1. Allen JJB, & Reznik SJ (2015). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a promising marker of depression vulnerability: summary and methodological considerations. Current Opinion in Psychology, 4, 93–97.
    1. Allen JJ, Urry HL, Hitt SK, & Coan JA (2004). The stability of resting frontal electroencephalographic asymmetry in depression. Psychophysiology, 41, 269–280.
    1. Ambrosini E, & Vallesi A (2016). Asymmetry in prefrontal resting-state EEG spectral power underlies individual differences in phasic and sustained cognitive control. Neuroimage, 124, 843–857.
    1. Arns M, Bruder G, Hegerl U, Spooner C, Palmer D, Etkin A, … Gordon E (2015). EEG alpha asymmetry as a gender-specific predictor of outcome to acute treatment with different antidepressant medications in the randomized iSPOT-D study. PLoS One, 127, 509–519. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2015.05.032
    1. Auerbach RP, Stewart JG, Stanton CH, Mueller EM, & Pizzagalli DA (2015). Emotion-processing biases and resting EEG activity in depressed adolescents. Depression and Anxiety, 32(9), 693–701.
    1. Baron RM, & Kenny D. a. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
    1. Barnhofer T, Duggan D, Crane C, Hepburn S, Fennell MJV, & Williams JMG (2007). Effects of meditation on frontal alpha-asymmetry in previously suicidal individuals. Neuroreport, 18(7), 709–712. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280d943cd
    1. Bigdely-Shamlo N, Mullen T, Kothe C, Su KM, & Robbins KA (2015). The PREP pipeline: standardized preprocessing for large-scale EEG analysis. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 9.
    1. Bismark AW, Moreno FA, Stewart JL, Towers DN, Coan JA, Oas J, Erickson RP, & Allen JJB (2010). Polymorphisms of the 5HT1A allele are linked to frontal brain electrical asymmetry. Biological Psychology, 83, 153–158
    1. Blackhart GC, Minnix JA, & Kline JP (2006). Can EEG asymmetry patterns predict future development of anxiety and depression? Biological Psychology, 72, 46–50. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.06.010
    1. Boksem MA, Kostermans E, Tops M, & De Cremer D (2012). Individual differences in asymmetric resting-state frontal cortical activity modulate ERPs and performance in a global-local attention task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 26(2), 51.
    1. Bruder GE, Stewart JW, Tenke CE, McGrath PJ, Leite P, Bhattacharya N, & Quitkin FM (2001). Electroencephalographic and perceptual asymmetry differences between responders and nonresponders to an SSRI antidepressant. Biological psychiatry, 49(5), 416–425.
    1. Bruder GE, Sedoruk JP, Stewart JW, McGrath PJ, Quitkin FM, & Tenke CE (2008). Electroencephalographic alpha measures predict therapeutic response to a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressant: pre- and post-treatment findings. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 1171–1177. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.10.009
    1. Bruzzone L, & Prieto DF (2000). Automatic analysis of the difference image for unsupervised change detection. Geoscience and Remote Sensing, IEEE Transactions on, 38, 1171–1182.
    1. Cacioppo JT (2004). Feelings and emotions: Roles for electrophysiological markers. Biological Psychology, 67, 235–243. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.009
    1. Chaumon M, Bishop DV, & Busch NA (2015). A practical guide to the selection of independent components of the electroencephalogram for artifact correction. Journal of neuroscience methods,, 250, 47–63.
    1. Çiçek M, & Nalçaci E (2001). Interhemispheric asymmetry of EEG alpha activity at rest and during the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: relations with performance. Biological psychology, 58(1), 75–88.
    1. Coan JA, & Allen JJB (2003). The state and trait nature of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion In Hugdahl K, Davidson RJ (Eds.), The Asymmetrical Brain, second ed. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 565–615.
    1. Coan JA, & Allen JJB (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biological Psychology, 67, 7–49.
    1. Coan JA, Allen JJ, & Harmon-Jones E (2001). Voluntary facial expression and hemispheric asymmetry over the frontal cortex. Psychophysiology, 38, 912–925.
    1. Coan JA, Allen JJB, & McKnight PE (2006). A capability model of individual differences in frontal EEG asymmetry. Biological Psychology, 72, 198–207.
    1. Cook EW, & Miller GA (1992). Digital filtering: Background and tutorial for psychophysiologists. Psychophysiology, 29, 350–362.
    1. Davidson RJ (1998). Anterior electrophysiological asymmetries, emotion, and depression: Conceptual and methodological conundrums. Psychophysiology, 35, 607–614.
    1. Davidson RJ (2004). What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect: perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry research. Biological Psychology, 6, 219–234. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.008
    1. Davidson RJ (1994). Asymmetric brain function, affective style, and psychopathology: The role of early experience and plasticity. Development and Psychopathology, 6(04), 741–758.
    1. Davidson RJ, Schaffer CE, & Saron C (1985). Effects of lateralized presentations of faces on self-reports of emotion and EEG asymmetry in depressed and non-depressed Subjects. Psychophysiology, 22, 353–364.
    1. Dawes RM, Faust D, & Meehl PE (1989). Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science, 243, 1668–1674.
    1. De Pascalis V, Cozzuto G, Caprara GV, & Alessandri G (2013). Relations among EEG-alpha asymmetry, BIS/BAS, and dispositional optimism. Biological Psychology, 94, 198–209.
    1. Delorme A, & Makeig S (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 9–21.
    1. Delorme A, Sejnowski T, & Makeig S (2007). Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higher-order statistics and independent component analysis. Neuroimage, 34, 1443–1449.
    1. Delorme A, Palmer J, Onton J, Oostenveld R, & Makeig S (2012). Independent EEG sources are dipolar. PloS one, 7, e30135.
    1. Dien J, Khoe W, & Mangun GR (2007). Evaluation of PCA and ICA of simulated ERPs: Promax vs. Infomax rotations. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 742–763.
    1. Dien J (2010). Evaluating two-step PCA of ERP data with geomin, infomax, oblimin, promax, and varimax rotations. Psychophysiology, 47, 170–183.
    1. Domschke K, Zwanzger P, Rehbein MA, Steinberg C, Knoke K, Dobel C, … & Pantev C (2015). Magnetoencephalographic correlates of emotional processing in major depression before and after pharmacological treatment. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 19(2), 1–9.
    1. Fachner J, Gold C, & Erkkilä J (2013). Music therapy modulates fronto-temporal activity in rest-EEG in depressed clients. Brain Topography, 26(2), 338–54. doi:10.1007/s10548-012-0254-x
    1. Fitzgibbon SP, Powers DM, Pope KJ, & Clark CR (2007). Removal of EEG noise and artifact using blind source separation. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 24, 232–243.
    1. Fox NA, & Davidson RJ (1986). Taste-elicited changes in facial signs of emotion and the asymmetry of brain electrical activity in human newborns. Neuropsychologia, 24, 417–422.
    1. Gable PA, Mechin N, Hicks J, & Adams DL (2015). Supervisory control system and frontal asymmetry: neurophysiological traits of emotion-based impulsivity. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 10(10), 1310–1315.
    1. Gollan JK, Hoxha D, Chihade D, Pflieger ME, Rosebrock L, & Cacioppo J (2014). Frontal alpha EEG asymmetry before and after behavioral activation treatment for depression. Biological Psychology, 99, 198–208. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.03.003
    1. Grimshaw GM, Foster JJ, & Corballis PM (2014). Frontal and parietal EEG asymmetries interact to predict attentional bias to threat. Brain and cognition, 90, 76–86.
    1. Groppe DM, Makeig S, & Kutas M (2009). Identifying reliable independent components via split-half comparisons. NeuroImage, 45, 1199–1211.
    1. Hagemann D (2004). Individual differences in anterior EEG asymmetry: Methodological problems and solutions. Biological Psychology, 67, 157–182.
    1. Hagemann D, Naumann E, & Thayer JF (2001). The quest for the EEG reference revisited: A glance from brain asymmetry research. Psychophysiology, 38, 847–857.
    1. Hagemann D Naumann E, Thayer JF, & Bartussek D (2002). Does resting electroencephalograph asymmetry reflect a trait?: An application of latent state-trait theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 619–641.
    1. Harmon-Jones E (2003). Clarifying the emotive functions of asymmetrical frontal cortical activity. Psychophysiology, 40, 838–848.
    1. Harmon-Jones E, & Allen JJ (1998). Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1310–1316.
    1. Harmon-Jones E, Gable PA, & Peterson CK (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biological Psychology, 84, 451–462.
    1. Harmon-Jones E, Peterson CK, & Harris CR (2009). Jealousy: novel methods and neural correlates. Emotion, 9, 113–117.
    1. Harmon-Jones E, & Sigelman J (2001). State anger and prefrontal brain activity: evidence that insult-related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 797–803.
    1. Harmon-Jones E, Vaughn-Scott K, Mohr S, Sigelman J, & Harmon-Jones C (2004). The effect of manipulated sympathy and anger on left and right frontal cortical activity. Emotion, 4, 1–7.
    1. Hatz F, Hardmeier M, Bousleiman H, Rueegg S, Schindler C, & Fuhr P (2015). Reliability of fully automated versus visually controlled pre-and post-processing of resting-state EEG. Clinical Neurophysiology, 126, 268–274.
    1. Heller W, Nitschke JB, Etienne MA, & Miller GA (1997). Patterns of regional brain activity differentiate types of anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(3), 376.
    1. Hoptman MJ, & Davidson RJ (1998). Baseline EEG asymmetries and performance on neuropsychological tasks. Neuropsychologia, 36(12), 1343–1353.
    1. Jung TP, Humphries C, Lee TW, Makeig S, McKeown MJ, Iragui V, & Sejnowski TJ (1998). Extended ICA Removes Artifacts from Electroencephalographic Recordings. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 10, 894–900.
    1. Jung TP, Humphries C, Lee TW, Makeig S, McKeown MJ, Iragui V, & Sejnowski TJ (1998, August). Removing electroencephalographic artifacts: comparison between ICA and PCA In Neural Networks for Signal Processing VIII, 1998. Proceedings of the 1998 IEEE Signal Processing Society Workshop (pp. 63–72). IEEE.
    1. Jung TP, Makeig S, Humphries C, Lee TW, Mckeown MJ, Iragui V, & Sejnowski TJ (2000). Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology, 37, 163–178.
    1. Kazdin AE (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 1–27. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432
    1. Killeen LA, & Teti DM (2012). Mothers’ frontal EEG asymmetry in response to infant emotion states and mother-infant emotional availability, emotional experience, and internalizing symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 9–21. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000629
    1. Koslov K, Mendes WB, Pajtas PE, & Pizzagalli DA (2011). Asymmetry in resting intracortical activity as a buffer to social threat. Psychological Science, 22(5), 641–649.
    1. MacKinnon DP, Lockhart G, Baraldi AN, & Gelfand LA (2013). Evaluating Treatment Mediators and Moderators. The Oxford Handbook of Research Strategies for Clinical Psychology, (April), 1–27. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199793549.013.0015
    1. Makeig S, Bell AJ, Jung TP, & Sejnowski TJ (1996). Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic data. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 145–151.
    1. Makeig S, Debener S, Onton J, & Delorme A (2004). Mining event-related brain dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 204–210.
    1. Mathersul D, Williams LM, Hopkinson PJ, & Kemp AH (2008). Investigating models of affect: relationships among EEG alpha asymmetry, depression, and anxiety. Emotion, 8, 560–572.
    1. Mathewson KE, Lleras A, Beck DM, Fabiani M, Ro T, & Gratton G (2011). Pulsed Out of Awareness: EEG Alpha Oscillations Represent a Pulsed-Inhibition of Ongoing Cortical Processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 99.
    1. McMenamin BW, Shackman AJ, Maxwell JS, Greischar LL, & Davidson RJ (2009). Validation of regression-based myogenic correction techniques for scalp and source-localized EEG. Psychophysiology, 46, 578–592.
    1. McMenamin BW, Shackman AJ, Maxwell JS, Bachhuber DR, Koppenhaver AM, Greischar LL, & Davidson RJ (2010). Validation of ICA-based myogenic artifact correction for scalp and source-localized EEG. Neuroimage, 49, 2416–2432.
    1. Meyer T, Smeets T, Giesbrecht T, Quaedflieg CW, Smulders FT, Meijer EH, & Merckelbach HL (2015). The role of frontal EEG asymmetry in post-traumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychology, 108, 62–77.
    1. Miller A, Fox NA, Cohn JF, Forbes EE, Sherrill JT, & Kovacs M (2002). Regional patterns of brain activity in adults with a history of childhood-onset depression: gender differences and clinical variability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 934–940.
    1. Miller GM, & Chapman JP (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40–48. doi:10.1037//0021-843X.110.1.40
    1. Miskovic V, & Schmidt LA (2010). Frontal brain electrical asymmetry and cardiac vagal tone predict biased attention to social threat. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 75(3), 332–338.
    1. Mognon A, Jovicich J, Bruzzone L, & Buiatti M (2011). ADJUST: An automatic EEG artifact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. Psychophysiology, 48, 229–240.
    1. Moscovitch D. a, Santesso DL, Miskovic V, McCabe RE, Antony MM, & Schmidt L. a. (2011). Frontal EEG asymmetry and symptom response to cognitive behavioral therapy in patients with social anxiety disorder. Biological Psychology, 87, 379–85. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.04.009
    1. Mullen T (2012). NITRC: CleanLine: Tool/Resource Info. Available online at: (Accessed March 22, 2015).
    1. Nusslock R, Shackman AJ, Harmon-jones E, Alloy LB, Coan JA, & Abramson LY (2011). Cognitive vulnerability and frontal brain asymmetry: Common predictors of first prospective depressive episode. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 497–503. doi:10.1037/a0022940
    1. Nusslock R, Walden K, & Harmon-Jones E (2015). Asymmetrical frontal cortical activity associated with differential risk for mood and anxiety disorder symptoms: An RDoC perspective. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98, 249–261.
    1. Onoda K, Okamoto Y, Shishida K, Hashizume A, Ueda K, Yamashita H, & Yamawaki S (2007). Anticipation of affective images and event-related desynchronization (ERD) of alpha activity: an MEG study. Brain research, 1151, 134–141.
    1. Onton J, Westerfield M, Townsend J, & Makeig S (2006). Imaging human EEG dynamics using independent component analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 808–822.
    1. Ota T, Toyoshima R, & Yamauchi T (1996). Measurements by biphasic changes of the alpha band amplitude as indicators of arousal level. International journal of psychophysiology, 24(1), 25–37.
    1. Papousek I, Reiser EM, Weber B, Freudenthaler HH, & Schulter G (2012). Frontal brain asymmetry and affective flexibility in an emotional contagion paradigm. Psychophysiology, 49, 489–98. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01324.x
    1. Pérez-Edgar K, Kujawa A, Nelson SK, Cole C, & Zapp DJ (2013). The relation between electroencephalogram asymmetry and attention biases to threat at baseline and under stress. Brain and cognition, 82(3), 337–343.
    1. Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, & Echallier JF (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology, 72, 184–187.
    1. Perrin F, Pernier J, Bertrand O, & Echallier JF (1990). Corrigenda. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 76, 565–566.
    1. Pizzagalli DA, Sherwood RJ, Henriques JB, & Davidson RJ (2005). Frontal brain asymmetry and reward responsiveness a source-localization study. Psychological Science, 16(10), 805–813.
    1. Preacher KJ, & Hayes AF (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731. doi:10.3758/BF03206553
    1. Quinn CR, Rennie CJ, Harris AW, & Kemp AH (2014). The impact of melancholia versus non-melancholia on resting-state, EEG alpha asymmetry: Electrophysiological evidence for depression heterogeneity. Psychiatry Research, 215, 614–617.
    1. Quraan M. a, Protzner AB, Daskalakis ZJ, Giacobbe P, Tang CW, Kennedy SH, … McAndrews MP (2014). EEG power asymmetry and functional connectivity as a marker of treatment effectiveness in DBS surgery for depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39, 1270–81. doi:10.1038/npp.2013.330
    1. Reid SA, Duke LM, & Allen JJB (1998). Resting frontal electroencephalographic asymmetry in depression: Inconsistencies suggest the need to identify mediating factors. Psychophysiology, 35, 389–404.
    1. Saletu B, Anderer P, & Saletu-Zyhlarz GM (2006). EEG topography and tomography (LORETA) in the classification and evaluation of the pharmacodynamics of psychotropic drugs. Clinical EEG and neuroscience,37(2), 66–80.
    1. Shackman AJ, McMenamin BW, Maxwell JS, Greischar LL, & Davidson RJ (2009). Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1500–1506.
    1. Schaffer CE, Davidson RJ, & Saron C (1983). Frontal and parietal electroencephalogram asymmetry in depressed and nondepressed subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 18, 753–762.
    1. Schmeichel BJ, Crowell A, & Harmon-Jones E (in press). Exercising self-control increases relative left frontal cortical activation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.
    1. Smith EE, Cavanagh JF, & Allen JJB (2013). Where it’s at: Estimating sources of resting-state frontal EEG alpha asymmetry. Psychophysiology, 50, S36.
    1. Smith EE, Zambrano-Vazquez L, & Allen JJ (2016). Patterns of alpha asymmetry in those with elevated worry, trait anxiety, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: A test of the worry and avoidance models of alpha asymmetry. Neuropsychologia, 85, 118–126.
    1. Stewart JL, Bismark AW, Towers DN, Coan JA, & Allen JJ (2010). Resting frontal EEG asymmetry as an endophenotype for depression risk: sex-specific patterns of frontal brain asymmetry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 502–512.
    1. Stewart JL, Coan JA, Towers DN, & Allen JJ (2011). Frontal EEG asymmetry during emotional challenge differentiates individuals with and without lifetime major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 129, 167–174.
    1. Stewart JL, Coan JA, Towers DN, & Allen JJ (2014). Resting and task-elicited prefrontal EEG alpha asymmetry in depression: Support for the capability model. Psychophysiology, 51, 446–455.
    1. Sutton SK, & Davidson RJ (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychological Science, 8, 204–210.
    1. Thibodeau R, Jorgensen RS, & Kim S (2006). Depression, anxiety, and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 715–729.
    1. Towers DN, & Allen JJ (2009). A better estimate of the internal consistency reliability of frontal EEG asymmetry scores. Psychophysiology, 46, 132–142.
    1. Tucker DM (1981). Lateral brain function, emotion, and conceptualization. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 19–46.
    1. Tullett AM, Harmon-Jones E, & Inzlicht M (2012). Right frontal cortical asymmetry predicts empathic reactions: Support for a link between withdrawal motivation and empathy. Psychophysiology, 49, 1145–1153.
    1. Velo JR, Stewart JL, Hasler BP, Towers DN, & Allen JJ (2012). Should it matter when we record? Time of year and time of day as factors influencing frontal EEG asymmetry. Biological Psychology, 91, 283–291.
    1. Viola FC, Thorne J, Edmonds B, Schneider T, Eichele T, & Debener S (2009). Semi-automatic identification of independent components representing EEG artifact. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 868–877.
    1. Wheeler RE, Davidson RJ, Tomarken AJ (1993). Frontal brain asymmetry and emotional reactivity: A biological substrate of affective style. Psychophysiology, 82–89.
    1. Winkler I, Haufe S, & Tangermann M (2011). Automatic classification of artifactual ICA-components for artifact removal in EEG signals. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 7, 1–15.
    1. Winkler I, Brandl S, Horn F, Waldburger E, Allefeld C, & Tangermann M (2014). Robust artifactual independent component classification for BCI practitioners. Journal of Neural Engineering, 11,35013–35022.
    1. Woo M, Kim S, Kim J, Petruzzello SJ, & Hatfield BD (2009). Examining the exercise-affect dose-response relationship: Does duration influence frontal EEG asymmetry? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72(2), 166–172. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.003

Source: PubMed

3
Abonneren