Walking in School-Aged Children in a Dual-Task Paradigm Is Related to Age But Not to Cognition, Motor Behavior, Injuries, or Psychosocial Functioning

Priska Hagmann-von Arx, Olivia Manicolo, Sakari Lemola, Alexander Grob, Priska Hagmann-von Arx, Olivia Manicolo, Sakari Lemola, Alexander Grob

Abstract

Age-dependent gait characteristics and associations with cognition, motor behavior, injuries, and psychosocial functioning were investigated in 138 typically developing children aged 6.7-13.2 years (M = 10.0 years). Gait velocity, normalized velocity, and variability were measured using the walkway system GAITRite without an additional task (single task) and while performing a motor or cognitive task (dual task). Assessment of children's cognition included tests for intelligence and executive functions; parents reported on their child's motor behavior, injuries, and psychosocial functioning. Gait variability (an index of gait regularity) decreased with increasing age in both single- and dual-task walking. Dual-task gait decrements were stronger when children walked in the motor compared to the cognitive dual-task condition and decreased with increasing age in both dual-task conditions. Gait alterations from single- to dual-task conditions were not related to children's cognition, motor behavior, injuries, or psychosocial functioning.

Keywords: dual-task walking; executive functions; gait; intelligence; psychosocial functioning; school-aged children.

Figures

FIGURE 1
FIGURE 1
Associations of age (in years) with gait velocity (A), normalized velocity (B), stride velocity variability (C), stride time variability (D), and stride length variability (E) in single-task walking and in the dual-task conditions digits and button. CV, coefficient of variation. ∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001.

References

    1. Abbruzzese L. D., Ashwini R. K., Bellows R., Figueroa K., Levy J., Lim E., et al. (2014). Effects of manual task complexity on gait parameters in school-aged children and adults. Gait Posture 40 658–663. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.07.017
    1. Adolph K. E., Vereijken B., Shrout P. E. (2003). What changes in infant walking and why. Child Dev. 74 475–497. 10.1111/1467-8624.7402011
    1. Al-Yahya E., Dawes H., Smith L., Dennis A., Howells D., Cockburn J. (2011). Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35 715–728. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.09.017
    1. Anderson P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 8 71–82. 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724
    1. Bejerot S., Humble M. B. (2013). Childhood clumsiness and peer victimization: a case-control study of psychiatric patients. BMC Psychiatry 13:68 10.1186/1471-244X-13-68
    1. Bejerot S., Plenty S., Humble A., Humble M. B. (2013). Poor motor skills: a risk marker for bully victimization. Aggress. Behav. 39 453–461. 10.1002/ab.21489
    1. Belmonti V., Cioni G., Berthoz A. (2013). Development of anticipatory orienting strategies and trajectory formation in goal-oriented locomotion. Exp. Brain Res. 227 131–147. 10.1007/s00221-013-3495-3
    1. Beurskens R., Bock O. (2012). Age-related deficits of dual-task walking: a review. Neural Plast. 2012:131608 10.1155/2012/131608
    1. Bloem B. R., Grimbergen Y. A. M., van Dijk G., Munneke M. (2006). The “posture second” strategy: a review of wrong priorities in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 248 196–204. 10.1016/j.jns.2006.05.010
    1. Bloemers F., Collard D., Paw M. C. A., Van Mechelen W., Twisk J., Verhagen E. (2012). Physical inactivity is a risk factor for physical activity-related injuries in children. Br. J. Sport Med. 46 669–674. 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090546
    1. Boonyong S., Siu K. C., van Donkelaar P., Chou L. S., Woollacott M. H. (2012). Development of postural control during gait in typically developing children: the effects of dual-task conditions. Gait Posture 35 428–434. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.11.002
    1. Bouffard M., Watkinson E. J., Thompson L. P., Causgrove Dunn J., Romanow S. K. E. (1996). A test of the activity deficit hypothesis with children with movement difficulties. Adapt. Phys. Act. Q. 13 61–73.
    1. Brandau H., Daghofer F. (2010). Elternfragebogen zur erfassung des unfallrisikos von jungen [Parental questionnaire for injury risk assessment of young boys]. Diagnostica 56 33–45. 10.1026/0012-1924/a000007
    1. Bril B., Ledebt A. (1998). Head coordination as a means to assist sensory integration in learning to walk. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22 555–563. 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00044-4
    1. Cairney J., Hay J. A., Faught B. E., Wade T. J., Corna L., Flouris S. (2005). Developmental coordination disorder, generalized self-efficacy toward physical activity, and participation in organized and free play activities. J. Pediatr. 147 515–520. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.05.013
    1. Cantell M., Smyth M., Ahonen T. (1994). Clumsiness in adolescence: educational, motor, and social outcomes of motor delay detected at 5 years. Adapt. Phys. Act. Q. 11 115–129.
    1. Cherng R. J., Liang L. Y., Chen Y. J., Chen J. Y. (2009). The effects of a motor and a cognitive concurrent task on walking in children with developmental coordination disorder. Gait Posture 29 204–207. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.08.003
    1. Cherng R. J., Liang L. Y., Hwang I. S., Chen J. Y. (2007). The effect of a concurrent task on the walking performance of preschool children. Gait Posture 26 231–237. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.09.004
    1. Daseking M., Petermann U., Petermann F. (2007). Intelligenzdiagnostik mit dem HAWIK-IV [Assessment of intelligence with the HAWIK-IV]. Kindh Entwickl. 16 250–259. 10.1026/0942-5403.16.4.250
    1. Dusing S. C., Thorpe D. E. (2007). A normative sample of temporal and spatial gait parameters in children using the GAITRite® electronic walkway. Gait Posture 25 135–139. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.06.003
    1. Ebersbach G., Dimitrijevic M. R., Poewe W. (1995). Influence of concurrent tasks on gait: a dual-task approach. Percept. Motor Skills 81 107–113. 10.2466/pms.1995.81.1.107
    1. Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., Buchner A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39 175–191. 10.3758/BF03193146
    1. Froehle A. W., Nahhas R. W., Sherwood R. J., Duren D. L. (2013). Age-related changes in spatiotemporal characteristics of gait accompany ongoing lower limb linear growth in late childhood and early adolescence. Gait Posture 38 14–19. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.10.005
    1. Gathercole S. E., Pickering S. J., Ambridge B., Wearing H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Dev. Psychol. 40 177–190. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177
    1. Gill S. V. (2015). Walking to the beat of their own drum: how children and adults meet timing constraints. PLoS ONE 10:e0127894 10.1371/journal.pone.0127894
    1. Hagmann-von Arx P., Grob A., Petermann F., Daseking M. (2012). Konkurrente validität des HAWIK-IV und der intelligence and development scales (IDS) [Concurrent validity of the HAWIK-IV and the intelligence and development scales (IDS)]. Z. Kinder Jug. Psych. 40 41–50. 10.1024/1422-4917/a000148
    1. Hagmann-von Arx P., Manicolo O., Perkinson-Gloor N., Weber P., Grob A., Lemola S. (2015). Gait in very preterm school-aged children in dual-task paradigms. PLoS ONE 10:e0144363 10.1371/journal.pone.0144363
    1. Hausdorff J. M. (2005). Gait variability: methods, modeling and meaning. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2:19 10.1186/1743-0003-2-19
    1. Hausdorff J. M. (2007). Gait dynamics, fractals and falls: finding meaning in the stride-to-stride fluctuations of human walking. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26 555–589. 10.1016/j.humov.2007.05.003
    1. Hausdorff J. M., Zemany L., Peng C. K., Goldberger A. L. (1999). Maturation of gait dynamics: stride-to-stride variability and its temporal organization in children. J. Appl. Physiol. 86 1040–1047.
    1. Hillman S. J., Stansfield B. W., Richardson A. M., Robb J. E. (2009). Development of temporal and distance parameters of gait in normal children. Gait Posture 29 81–85. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.06.012
    1. Hof A. L. (1996). Scaling gait data to body size. Gait Posture 4 222–223. 10.1016/0966-6362(95)01057-2
    1. Holm I., Tveter A. T., Frederiksen P. M., Vollestad N. (2009). A normative sample of gait and hopping on one leg parameters in children 7-12 years of age. Gait Posture 27 91–96. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.09.016
    1. Huang H. J., Mercer V. S. (2001). Dual-task methodology: applications in studies of cognitive and motor performance in adults and children. Pediatr. Phys. Ther. 13 133–140. 10.1097/00001577-200113030-00005
    1. Huang H. J., Mercer V. S., Thorpe D. E. (2003). Effects of different concurrent cognitive tasks on temporal-distance gait variables in children. Pediatr. Phys. Ther. 15 105–113. 10.1097/01.PEP.0000067886.96352.6B
    1. Hung Y. C., Meredith G. S., Gill S. V. (2013). Influence of dual task constraints during walking for children. Gait Posture 38 450–454. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.01.009
    1. Iverson J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. J. Child Lang. 37 229–261. 10.1017/S0305000909990432
    1. Kahneman D. (1973). Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
    1. Karasik L. B., Tamis-LeMonda C. S., Adolph K. E. (2011). Transition from crawling to walking and infants’ actions with objects and people. Child Dev. 82 1199–1209. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01595.x
    1. Katz-Leurer M., Rotem H., Keren O., Meyer S. (2011). Effect of concurrent cognitive tasks on gait features among children post-severe traumatic brain injury and typically-developed controls. Brain Inj. 25 581–586. 10.3109/02699052.2011.572943
    1. Katz-Leurer M., Rotem H., Meyer S. (2013). Effects of concurrent cognitive tasks on temporo-spatial parameters of gait among children with cerebral palsy and typically developed controls. Dev. Neurorehabil. 17 363–367. 10.3109/17518423.2013.810676
    1. Kressig R. W., Beauchet O. (2006). European GAITRite network group, guidelines for clinical applications of spatio-temporal gait analysis in older adults. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 18 174–176. 10.1007/BF03327437
    1. Leitner Y., Barak R., Giladi N., Peretz C., Eshel R., Gruendlinger L., et al. (2007). Gait in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of methylphenidate and dual tasking. J. Neurol. 254 1330–1338. 10.1007/s00415-006-0522-3
    1. Lindenberger U., Marsiske M., Baltes P. (2000). Memorizing while walking: increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychol. Aging 15 417–436. 10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.417
    1. Luciana M. (2003). Practitioner review: computerized assessment of neuropsychological function in children: clinical and research applications of the cambridge neuropsychological testing automated battery (CANTAB). J. Child Psychol. Psychiirty 44 649–663. 10.1111/1469-7610.00152
    1. Lythgo N., Wilson C., Galea M. (2009). Basic gait and symmetry measures for primary school-aged children and young adults whilst walking barefoot and with shoes. Gait Posture 30 502–506. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.07.119
    1. Lythgo N., Wilson C., Galea M. (2011). Basic gait and symmetry measures for primary school-aged children and young adults II. Walking at slow, free and fast speed. Gait Posture 33 29–35. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.09.017
    1. Manicolo O., Grob A., Lemola S., Hagmann-von Arx P. (2016). Age-related decline of gait variability in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: support for the maturational delay hypothesis. Gait Posture 44 245–249. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.12.012
    1. Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki H., Howerter A., Wager T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lope” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41 49–100. 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
    1. Pashler H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol. Bull. 116 220–244. 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220
    1. Petermann F. (2008). Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd Edn. Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment.
    1. Petermann F., Petermann U. (2011). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV). Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment.
    1. Piek J. P., Barrett N. C., Allen L. S. R., Jones A., Louise M. (2006a). The relationship between bullying and self-worth in children with movement coordination problems. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 75 453–463. 10.1348/000709904X24573
    1. Piek J. P., Baynam G. B., Barrett N. C. (2006b). The relationship between fine and gross motor ability, self-perception and self-worth in children and adolescents. Hum. Mov. Sci. 25 65–75. 10.1016/j.humov.2005.10.011
    1. Ravens-Sieberer U., Gosch A., Rajmil L., Erhart M., Bruil J., Power M., et al. (2008). The KIDSCREEN-52 quality of life measure for children and adolescents: psychometric results from a cross-cultural survey in 13 European countries. Value Health 4 645–658. 10.1111/j.1524-4733.2007.00291.x
    1. Schaefer S., Lövdén M., Wieckhorst B., Lindenberger U. (2010). Cognitive performance is improved while walking: differences in cognitive-sensorimotor couplings between children and young adults. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 7 371–389. 10.1080/17405620802535666
    1. Schoemaker M. M., Kalverboer A. F. (1994). Social and affective problems of children who are clumsy: how early do they begin. Adapt. Phys. Act. Q. 11 130–140.
    1. Skinner R. A., Piek J. P. (2001). Psychosocial implications of poor motor coordination in children and adolescents. Hum. Mov. Sci. 20 73–94. 10.1016/S0167-9457(01)00029-X
    1. Speltz M. L., Gonzales N., Sulzbacher S., Quan L. (1990). Assessment of injury risk in young children: a preliminary study of the injury behavior checklist. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 15 373–383. 10.1093/jpepsy/15.3.373
    1. Springer S., Giladi N., Peretz C., Yogev G., Simon E. S., Hausdorff J. M. (2006). Dual-tasking effects on gait variability: the role of aging, falls, and executive function. Mov. Disord. 21 950–957. 10.1002/mds.20848
    1. Storvold G. V., Aarethun K., Bratberg G. H. (2013). Age for onset of walking and prewalking strategies. Early Hum. Dev. 89 655–659. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.04.010
    1. Sutherland D. H., Olshen R., Biden E. N., Wyatt M. P. (1988). The Development of Mature Walking. Oxford: MacKeith.
    1. Thorpe D. E., Dusing S. C., Moore C. G. (2005). Repeatability of temporospatial gait measures in children using the GAITRite electronic walkway. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 86 2342–2346. 10.1016/j.apmr.2005.07.301
    1. Tombu M., Jolicoeur P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. 29 3–18. 10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.3
    1. Wickens C. D. (1991). “Processing resources in attention,” in Multiple-Task Performance, ed. Damos D. L. (Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis; ), 3–34.
    1. Woollacott M., Shumway-Cook A. (2002). Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 16 1–14. 10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00156-4
    1. Yang Y. R., Chen Y. C., Lee C. S., Cheng S. J., Wang R. Y. (2007). Dual-task-related gait changes in individuals with stroke. Gait Posture 25 185–190. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.03.007
    1. Yogev-Seligmann G., Hausdorff J. M., Giladi N. (2008). The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov. Disord. 23 329–342. 10.1002/mds.21720
    1. Zwicker J. G., Missiuna C., Harris S. R., Boyd L. A. (2012). Developmental coordination disorder: a review and update. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 16 573–581. 10.1016/j.ejpn.2012.05.005

Source: PubMed

3
Abonneren