β-amyloid disrupts human NREM slow waves and related hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation

Bryce A Mander, Shawn M Marks, Jacob W Vogel, Vikram Rao, Brandon Lu, Jared M Saletin, Sonia Ancoli-Israel, William J Jagust, Matthew P Walker, Bryce A Mander, Shawn M Marks, Jacob W Vogel, Vikram Rao, Brandon Lu, Jared M Saletin, Sonia Ancoli-Israel, William J Jagust, Matthew P Walker

Abstract

Independent evidence associates β-amyloid pathology with both non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep disruption and memory impairment in older adults. However, whether the influence of β-amyloid pathology on hippocampus-dependent memory is, in part, driven by impairments of NREM slow wave activity (SWA) and associated overnight memory consolidation is unknown. Here we show that β-amyloid burden in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) correlates significantly with the severity of impairment in NREM SWA generation. Moreover, reduced NREM SWA generation was further associated with impaired overnight memory consolidation and impoverished hippocampal-neocortical memory transformation. Furthermore, structural equation models revealed that the association between mPFC β-amyloid pathology and impaired hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation was not direct, but instead statistically depended on the intermediary factor of diminished NREM SWA. By linking β-amyloid pathology with impaired NREM SWA, these data implicate sleep disruption as a mechanistic pathway through which β-amyloid pathology may contribute to hippocampus-dependent cognitive decline in the elderly.

Figures

Figure 1. Aβ, NREM SWA, and memory…
Figure 1. Aβ, NREM SWA, and memory retention measures in three sample subjects
[11C]PIB-PET DVR images demonstrating Aβ deposition (a), NREM SWA and associated localized slow wave source (b), proportion of NREM SWA 0.6–1Hz at FZ&CZ derivations (c), and overnight memory retention (long delay recognition testing – short delay recognition testing; d) measures in three sample participants with low mPFC PIB DVR (left column), intermediate mPFC PIB DVR (middle column), and high mPFC PIB DVR (right column). PIB-PET mPFC region of interest (ROI) is outlined in white (a) and the mPFC EEG derivations are outlined in black (b), with accompanying source analysis (thresholded at ±7) verifying mPFC overlap across PIB-PET and EEG ROIs (see Methods and Supplementary Fig. 2). Prop. denotes proportion, PIB denotes Pittsburgh compound B, PET denotes positron emission tomography, DVR denotes distribution volume ratio referenced against the whole cerebellum, mPFC denotes medial prefrontal cortex, and [HR–FAR–LR] denotes [hit rate to originally studied word pairs – false alarm rate to new, unstudied words – false alarm rate to originally studied word pairs].
Figure 2. Associations between Aβ, NREM SWA,…
Figure 2. Associations between Aβ, NREM SWA, and memory retention measures
Associations between LN transformed [11C]PIB-PET DVR measured mPFC Aβ deposition, mPFC relative SWA, mPFC SW density, and overnight memory retention. Interaction plots of two-way, repeated measures ANCOVAs, which revealed that Aβ burden was associated with lower relative mPFC NREM SWA and SW density 0.6–1Hz and higher mPFC NREM SWA and SW density at 1–4Hz (parameter estimates for each frequency bin plotted in a) for SWA and c) for SW density. mPFC Aβ burden was also negatively associated with proportion of mPFC NREM SWA 0.6–1Hz (b). mPFC NREM SWA 0.6–1Hz, in turn, positively predicted overnight memory retention (d). %PTOT denotes percentage of total spectral power (0.6–50Hz), Prop. denotes proportion, PIB denotes Pittsburgh compound B, PET denotes positron emission tomography, DVR denotes distribution volume ratio referenced against the cerebellum, mPFC denotes medial prefrontal cortex, and [HR–FAR–LR] denotes [hit rate to originally studied word pairs – false alarm rate to new, unstudied words – false alarm rate to originally studied word pairs].
Figure 3. Associations between NREM SWA, retrieval-related…
Figure 3. Associations between NREM SWA, retrieval-related hippocampus activation, and memory retention
(a) Negative association between proportion of mPFC SWA 0.6–1Hz and left hippocampal activation greater during successful associative episodic retrieval than correct rejection of novel words (Hits-Correct Rejections); 8mm-sphere ROI: [x=−22, y=−14, z=−12; x=−23, y=−15, z=−16 in mni coordinates]. Activations were inclusively masked by hippocampal anatomy and displayed and considered significant at the voxel level ofP<0.05 family-wise error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons within the a priori hippocampal region of interest. Peak effects were detected at [x=−24, y=−16, z=−14]. Hot colors represent the extent of the negative association between hippocampal activation and proportion of SWA 0.6–1Hz. (b) Negative association between overnight memory retention and the average contrast estimate of significant hippocampal voxels, extracted using marsbar. au denotes arbitrary units, prop. denotes proportion, and [HR–FAR–LR] denotes [hit rate to originally studied word pairs – false alarm rate to new, unstudied words – false alarm rate to originally studied word pairs].
Figure 4. Path models linking Aβ, NREM…
Figure 4. Path models linking Aβ, NREM SWA, retrieval-related hippocampus activation, and memory retention
Path analysis models examining the relative contributions of [11C]PIB-PET DVR measured mPFC β-amyloid (Aβ) deposition, proportion of mPFC NREM SWA 0.6–1Hz, and retrieval-related hippocampal (HC) activation to overnight memory retention (long delay recognition testing — short delay recognition testing) in three hypothesized models (a–c). Values represent standardized regression weights. Models were estimated and model fit for the sleep and HC-independent model (a, BIC = 29.640; RMR = 0.021; GFI = 0.858), the sleep-independent and HC-dependent model (b, BIC = 29.131; RMR = 0.021; GFI = 0.873), and the sleep-dependent model (c, BIC = 24.676; RMR = 0.006; GFI = 0.931) were compared against a saturated model (BIC = 30.910; RMR = 0.000; GFI = 1.000), and an independence model (BIC = 30.747; RMR = 0.046; GFI = 0.617). * denotes path significance at P<0.05.

References

    1. Boyle PA, et al. Much of late life cognitive decline is not due to common neurodegenerative pathologies. Ann Neurol. 2013
    1. Buckner RL, et al. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7709–7717.
    1. Jack CR, Jr, et al. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:119–128.
    1. Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Becker A, Sperling R, Johnson KA. In vivo characterization of the early states of the amyloid-beta network. Brain. 2013;136:2239–2252.
    1. Spires-Jones TL, Hyman BT. The intersection of amyloid beta and tau at synapses in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuron. 2014;82:756–771.
    1. Mormino EC, et al. Episodic memory loss is related to hippocampal-mediated beta-amyloid deposition in elderly subjects. Brain. 2009;132:1310–1323.
    1. Oh H, Jagust WJ. Frontotemporal network connectivity during memory encoding is increased with aging and disrupted by beta-amyloid. J Neurosci. 2013;33:18425–18437.
    1. Backhaus J, et al. Midlife decline in declarative memory consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep. Learn Mem. 2007;14:336–341.
    1. Carrier J, et al. Sleep slow wave changes during the middle years of life. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33:758–766.
    1. Dijk DJ, Beersma DG, van den Hoofdakker RH. All night spectral analysis of EEG sleep in young adult and middle-aged male subjects. Neurobiol Aging. 1989;10:677–682.
    1. Mander BA, et al. Prefrontal atrophy, disrupted NREM slow waves, and impaired hippocampal-dependent memory in aging. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:357–364.
    1. Van Cauter E, Leproult R, Plat L. Age-related changes in slow wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men. Jama. 2000;284:861–868.
    1. Westerberg CE, et al. Concurrent Impairments in Sleep and Memory in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012:1–11.
    1. Marshall L, Helgadottir H, Molle M, Born J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature. 2006;444:610–613.
    1. Murphy M, et al. Source modeling sleep slow waves. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:1608–1613.
    1. Hita-Yanez E, Atienza M, Cantero JL. Polysomnographic and subjective sleep markers of mild cognitive impairment. Sleep. 2013;36:1327–1334.
    1. Prinz PN, et al. Sleep, EEG and mental function changes in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Neurobiol Aging. 1982;3:361–370.
    1. Liguori C, et al. Orexinergic system dysregulation, sleep impairment, and cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71:1498–1505.
    1. Kang JE, et al. Amyloid-beta dynamics are regulated by orexin and the sleep-wake cycle. Science. 2009;326:1005–1007.
    1. Roh JH, et al. Disruption of the sleep-wake cycle and diurnal fluctuation of beta-amyloid in mice with Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Sci Transl Med. 2012;4:150ra122.
    1. Spira AP, et al. Self-reported Sleep and beta-Amyloid Deposition in Community-Dwelling Older Adults. JAMA Neurol. 2013
    1. Xie L, et al. Sleep drives metabolite clearance from the adult brain. Science. 2013;342:373–377.
    1. Chauvette S, Seigneur J, Timofeev I. Sleep oscillations in the thalamocortical system induce long-term neuronal plasticity. Neuron. 2012;75:1105–1113.
    1. Kim H. Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: a meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies. Neuroimage. 2011;54:2446–2461.
    1. Riedner BA, et al. Sleep homeostasis and cortical synchronization: III. A high-density EEG study of sleep slow waves in humans. Sleep. 2007;30:1643–1657.
    1. Pascual-Marqui RD. Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol. 2002;24(Suppl D):5–12.
    1. Buzsaki G. The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. Cerebral Cortex. 1996;6:81–92.
    1. Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:119–130.
    1. Diekelmann S, Born J. The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:114–126.
    1. Takashima A, et al. Declarative memory consolidation in humans: a prospective functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:756–761.
    1. Walker MP. The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2009;1156:168–197.
    1. Stage FK, Carter HC, Nora A. Path analysis: An introduction and analysis of a decade of research. Journal of Educational Research. 2004;98:5–12.
    1. Bozdogan H. Model Selection and Akaike Information Criterion (Aic) - the General-Theory and Its Analytical Extensions. Psychometrika. 1987;52:345–370.
    1. Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. The Guilford Press; New York, N.Y: 2011.
    1. Raftery AE. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology 1995. 1995;25:111–163.
    1. Steriade M, Contreras D, Curro Dossi R, Nunez A. The slow (< 1 Hz) oscillation in reticular thalamic and thalamocortical neurons: scenario of sleep rhythm generation in interacting thalamic and neocortical networks. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3284–3299.
    1. Steriade M, Nunez A, Amzica F. A novel slow (< 1 Hz) oscillation of neocortical neurons in vivo: depolarizing and hyperpolarizing components. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3252–3265.
    1. Kurup P, et al. Abeta-mediated NMDA receptor endocytosis in Alzheimer’s disease involves ubiquitination of the tyrosine phosphatase STEP61. J Neurosci. 2010;30:5948–5957.
    1. Snyder EM, et al. Regulation of NMDA receptor trafficking by amyloid-beta. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1051–1058.
    1. Cavdar S, et al. The pathways connecting the hippocampal formation, the thalamic reuniens nucleus and the thalamic reticular nucleus in the rat. J Anat. 2008;212:249–256.
    1. Ooms S, et al. Effect of 1 Night of Total Sleep Deprivation on Cerebrospinal Fluid beta-Amyloid 42 in Healthy Middle-Aged Men: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Neurol. 2014
    1. Ju YE, Lucey BP, Holtzman DM. Sleep and Alzheimer disease pathology--a bidirectional relationship. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10:115–119.
    1. Cricco M, Simonsick EM, Foley DJ. The impact of insomnia on cognitive functioning in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:1185–1189.
    1. Yaffe K, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing, hypoxia, and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older women. JAMA. 2011;306:613–619.
    1. Lim AS, et al. Modification of the relationship of the apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele to the risk of Alzheimer disease and neurofibrillary tangle density by sleep. JAMA Neurol. 2013;70:1544–1551.
    1. Brett M, Anton JL, Valabregue R, Poline JB. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract]. NeuroImage; 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain; Sendai, Japan. 2002.
    1. Mander BA, et al. Impaired Prefrontal Sleep Spindle Regulation of Hippocampal-Dependent Learning in Older Adults. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:3301–3309.
    1. Yesavage JA, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17:37–49.
    1. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198.
    1. Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B. California verbal learning test. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 2000.
    1. Wechsler D. Wechsler memory scale-revised. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1987.
    1. Reitan RM. Validity of the trail-making test as an indication of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills. 1958;8:271–276.
    1. Zec RF. The stroop color-word test: A paradigm for procedural learning. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1986;1:274–275.
    1. Young T, Peppard PE, Gottlieb DJ. Epidemiology of obstructive sleep apnea: a population health perspective. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2002;165:1217–1239.
    1. Tan X, Campbell IG, Feinberg I. Internight reliability and benchmark values for computer analyses of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) and REM EEG in normal young adult and elderly subjects. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112:1540–1552.
    1. Zheng H, et al. Sources of variability in epidemiological studies of sleep using repeated nights of in-home polysomnography: SWAN Sleep Study. J Clin Sleep Med. 2012;8:87–96.
    1. Cohen DA, Robertson EM. Preventing interference between different memory tasks. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:953–955.
    1. Jack CR, Jr, et al. Neurology. 2013;80:890–896.
    1. Villemagne VL, et al. Amyloid beta deposition, neurodegeneration, and cognitive decline in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease: a prospective cohort study. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:357–367.
    1. Elman JA, et al. Neural compensation in older people with brain amyloid-beta deposition. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:1316–1318.
    1. Oh H, Madison C, Haight TJ, Markley C, Jagust WJ. Effects of age and beta-amyloid on cognitive changes in normal elderly people. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:2746–2755.
    1. Desikan RS, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980.
    1. Bradshaw EM, et al. CD33 Alzheimer’s disease locus: altered monocyte function and amyloid biology. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:848–850.
    1. Hedden T, et al. Cognitive profile of amyloid burden and white matter hyperintensities in cognitively normal older adults. J Neurosci. 2012;32:16233–16242.
    1. Hedden T, et al. Failure to modulate attentional control in advanced aging linked to white matter pathology. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22:1038–1051.
    1. Lieberman MD, Cunningham WA. Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4:423–428.
    1. Lancaster JL, et al. Bias between MNI and talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Human Brain Mapping. 2007;28:1194–1205.
    1. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques an scoring system of sleep stages in human subjects. UCLA Brain Information Services; Los Angeles: 1968.
    1. Mander BA, Santhanam S, Saletin JM, Walker MP. Wake deterioration and sleep restoration of human learning. Curr Biol. 2011;21:R183–184.
    1. Saletin JM, van der Helm E, Walker MP. Structural brain correlates of human sleep oscillations. Neuroimage. 2013;83:658–668.
    1. Mazziotta J, et al. A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM) Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2001;356:1293–1322.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonneren