Consistency of Hearing Aid Setting Preference in Simulated Real-World Environments: Implications for Trainable Hearing Aids

Els Walravens, Gitte Keidser, Louise Hickson, Els Walravens, Gitte Keidser, Louise Hickson

Abstract

Trainable hearing aids let users fine-tune their hearing aid settings in their own listening environment: Based on consistent user-adjustments and information about the acoustic environment, the trainable aids will change environment-specific settings to the user's preference. A requirement for effective fine-tuning is consistency of preference for similar settings in similar environments. The aim of this study was to evaluate consistency of preference for settings differing in intensity, gain-frequency slope, and directionality when listening in simulated real-world environments and to determine if participants with more consistent preferences could be identified based on profile measures. A total of 52 adults (63-88 years) with hearing varying from normal to a moderate sensorineural hearing loss selected their preferred setting from pairs differing in intensity (3 or 6 dB), gain-frequency slope (±1.3 or ± 2.7 dB/octave), or directionality (omnidirectional vs. cardioid) in four simulated real-world environments: traffic noise, a monologue in traffic noise at 5 dB signal-to-noise ratio, and a dialogue in café noise at 5 and at 0 dB signal-to-noise ratio. Forced-choice comparisons were made 10 times for each combination of pairs of settings and environment. Participants also completed nine psychoacoustic, cognitive, and personality measures. Consistency of preference, defined by a setting preferred at least 9 out of 10 times, varied across participants. More participants obtained consistent preferences for larger differences between settings and less difficult environments. The profile measures did not predict consistency of preference. Trainable aid users could benefit from counselling to ensure realistic expectations for particular adjustments and listening situations.

Keywords: fine-tuning; hearing loss; normally hearing.

Figures

Figure 1.
Figure 1.
The Spread in Binaural Average Hearing Loss Represented by the Four-Frequency Average Hearing Loss, and the Slope, That Is, the Difference Between the Average Thresholds Across 250, 500, and 1000 Hz (Low-Frequency Average) and Across 2, 3, and 4 kHz (High-Frequency Average).
Figure 2.
Figure 2.
The Levels of Speech (Full Line) and Noise (Dashed Line) Across the Four Listening Environments in Third Octave Bands (dB SPL Long-Term Average): Traffic Noise, Monologue in Traffic Noise at 5 dB SNR, Dialogue in Café Noise at 5 dB SNR and at 0 dB SNR.
Figure 3.
Figure 3.
The Number of Consistent Preferences Obtained by Participants (n = 52) Across the Large Intensity and Gain-Frequency Slope Differences (Top) and Small Directionality, Intensity, and Gain-Frequency Slope Differences (Bottom) in the Different Listening Environments.
Figure 4.
Figure 4.
The Number of Participants With a Consistent Preference for the Five Pairs of HA Settings Across the Four Listening Environments From Easiest to Most Difficult: Traffic Noise, Monologue in Traffic Noise, Dialogue in Café Noise at 5 dB SNR and 0 dB SNR.

References

    1. American National Standards Institute. (1998). Methods for calculation of the speech intelligibility index (ANSI S3.5-1997).
    1. Anderson M. C., Arehart K. H., Souza P. E. (2018). Survey of current practice in the fitting and fine-tuning of common signal-processing features in hearing aids for adults. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 29(2), 118–124. 10.3766/jaaa.16107
    1. Athalye S. P. (2010). Factors affecting speech recognition in noise and hearing loss in adults with a wide variety of auditory capabilities [PhD thesis]. Institute of Sound and Vibration Research, University of Southampton,
    1. Bentler R. A., Chiou L.-K. (2006). Digital noise reduction: An overview. Trends in Amplification, 10(2), 67–82. 10.1177/1084713806289514
    1. Best V., Keidser G., Freeston K., Buchholz J. M. (2016). A dynamic speech comprehension test for assessing real-world listening ability. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 27(7), 515–526. 10.3766/jaaa.15089
    1. Byrne D., Cotton S. (1988). Evaluation of the National Acoustic Laboratories’ new hearing aid selection procedure. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31(2), 178–186. 10.1044/jshr.3102.178
    1. Caswell-Midwinter B., Whitmer W. M. (2019). Discrimination of gain increments in speech-shaped noises. Trends in Hearing, 23, 2331216518820220 10.1177/2331216518820220
    1. Chasin M. (2017). Overview of smartphone control of hearing aids. Canadian Audiologist, 4(4).
    1. Cox R. M., Alexander G. C., Taylor I. M., Gray G. A. (1997). The contour test of loudness perception. Ear and Hearing, 18(5), 388–400. 10.1097/00003446-199710000-00004
    1. Daneman M., Carpenter P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6
    1. De Beuckelaer A., Kampen J. K., Van Trijp J. C. M. (2013). An empirical assessment of the cross-national measurement validity of graded paired comparisons. Quality & Quantity, 47(2), 1063–1076. 10.1007/s11135-011-9583-1
    1. Dillon H., Zakis J. A., McDermott H. J., Keidser G., Dreschler W. A., Convery E. (2006). The trainable hearing aid: What will it do for clients and clinicians? Hearing Journal, 59(4), 30–34, 36. 10.1097/01.HJ.0000286694.20964.4a
    1. Dreschler W. A., Keidser G., Convery E., Dillon H. (2008). Client-based adjustments of hearing aid gain: The effect of different control configurations. Ear and Hearing, 29(2), 214–227. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31816453a6
    1. Edwards B. W., Hou Z., Struck C. J., Dharan P. (1998). Signal processing algorithms for a new, software-based, digital hearing device. Hearing Journal, 51(9), 44–52.
    1. Gosling S. D., Rentfrow P. J., Swann W. B. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528. 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1
    1. Hansen M. (2006). Lehre und Ausbildung in Psychoakustik mit psylab: freie Software für psychoakustische Experimente [Education in psychoacoustics with psylab: free software for psychoacoustic experiments]. Paper presented at the Fortschritte der Akustik - DAGA 2006, Braunschweig.
    1. Helfer K. S., Wilber L. A. (1990). Hearing loss, aging, and speech perception in reverberation and noise. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33(1), 149–155. 10.1044/jshr.3301.149
    1. Holube I., Fredelake S., Vlaming M., Kollmeier B. (2010). Development and analysis of an International Speech Test Signal (ISTS). International Journal of Audiology, 49(12), 891–903. 10.3109/14992027.2010.506889
    1. Hothorn T., Bretz F., Westfall P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical Journal, 50(3), 346–363. 10.1002/bimj.200810425
    1. Jepsen M. L., Dau T. (2011). Characterizing auditory processing and perception in individual listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(1), 262–281. 10.1121/1.3518768
    1. Keidser G. (1995). The relationship between listening conditions and alternative amplification schemes for multiple memory hearing aids. Ear and Hearing, 16(6), 575–586. 10.1097/00003446-199512000-00004
    1. Keidser G., Brew C., Brewer S., Dillon H., Grant F., Storey L. (2005). The preferred response slopes and two-channel compression ratios in twenty listening conditions by hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners and their relationship to the acoustic input. International Journal of Audiology, 44(11), 656–670. 10.1080/14992020500266803
    1. Keidser G., Dillon H., Convery E. (2008). The effect of the base line response on self-adjustments of hearing aid gain. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(3), 1668–1681. 10.1121/1.2951500
    1. Keidser G., Dillon H., Flax M., Ching T. Y. C., Brewer S. (2011). The NAL-NL2 prescription procedure. Audiology Research, 1(e24), 88–90. 10.4081/audiores.2011.e24
    1. Keidser G., Grant F. (2001). Comparing loudness normalization (IHAFF) with speech intelligibility maximization (NAL-NL1) when implemented in a two-channel device. Ear and Hearing, 22(6), 501–515. 10.1097/00003446-200112000-00006
    1. Kincaid J. P., Fishburne R. P., Jr., Rogers R. L., Chissom B. S. (1975). Derivation of New Readability Formulas (Automated Readability Index, Fog Count and Flesch Reading Ease Formula) for Navy Enlisted Personnel,
    1. Kuk F. K., Pape N. M. C. (1992). The reliability of a modified simplex procedure in hearing aid frequency-response selection. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35(2), 418–429. 10.1044/jshr.3502.418
    1. Larsby B., Arlinger S. (1998). A method for evaluating temporal, spectral and combined temporal-spectral resolution of hearing. Scandinavian Audiology, 27(1), 3–12. 10.1080/010503998419641
    1. Lentz J. J., Leek M. R. (2003). Spectral shape discrimination by hearing-impaired and normal-hearing listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113(3), 1604–1616. 10.1121/1.1553461
    1. Lister J. J., Roberts R. A., Lister F. L. (2011). An adaptive clinical test of temporal resolution: Age effects. International Journal of Audiology, 50(6), 367–374. 10.3109/14992027.2010.551218
    1. Lunner T. (2003). Cognitive function in relation to hearing aid use. International Journal of Audiology, 42(Suppl 1), S49–S58. 10.3109/14992020309074624
    1. McShefferty D., Whitmer W. M., Akeroyd M. A. (2015). The just-noticeable difference in speech-to-noise ratio. Trends in Hearing, 19, 1–9 10.1177/2331216515572316
    1. McShefferty D., Whitmer W. M., Akeroyd M. A. (2016). The just-meaningful difference in speech-to-noise ratio. Trends in Hearing, 20 10.1177/2331216515626570
    1. Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki A. H., Howerter A., Wager T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
    1. Moore B. C. J., Sęk A. (2013). Comparison of the CAM2 and NAL-NL2 hearing aid fitting methods. Ear and Hearing, 34(1), 83–95. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182650adf
    1. Morris N., Jones D. M. (1990). Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 111–121. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02349.xronn
    1. Nasreddine Z. S., Phillips N. A., Bedirian V., Charbonneau S., Whitehead V., Collin I., Cummings J. L., Chertkow H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695–699. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x
    1. Neher T. (2014). Relating hearing loss and executive functions to hearing aid users’ preference for, and speech recognition with, different combinations of binaural noise reduction and microphone directionality. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 391 10.3389/fnins.2014.00391
    1. Neher T., Grimm G., Hohmann V., Kollmeier B. (2014). Do hearing loss and cognitive function modulate benefit from different binaural noise-reduction settings? Ear and Hearing, 35(3), e52–62. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000003
    1. Oreinos C. (2015). Virtual acoustic environments for the evaluation of hearing devices (PhD Thesis). Macquarie University.
    1. Oreinos C., Buchholz J. M. (2015). Objective analysis of ambisonics for hearing aid applications: Effect of listener’s head, room reverberation, and directional microphones. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 137(6), 3447–3465. 10.1121/1.4919330
    1. Oreinos C., Buchholz J. M. (2016). Evaluation of loudspeaker-based virtual sound environments for testing directional hearing aids. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 27(7), 541–556. 10.3766/jaaa.15094
    1. Pearsons K. S., Bennett R. L., Sanford F. (1977). Speech levels in various noise environments,
    1. Pichora-Fuller M. K., Goy H., Van Lieshout P. (2010). Effect on speech intelligibility of changes in speech production influenced by instructions and communication environments. Seminars in Hearing, 31(02), 077–094. 10.1055/s-0030-1252100
    1. Rönnberg J., Arlinger S., Lyxell B., Kinnefors C. (1989). Visual evoked potentials: Relation to adult speechreading and cognitive function. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32(4), 725–735. 10.1044/jshr.3204.725
    1. Smeds K., Wolters F., Rung M. (2015). Estimation of signal-to-noise ratios in realistic sound scenarios. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 26(2), 183–196. 10.3766/jaaa.26.2.7
    1. Thielemans T., Pans D., Chenault M., Anteunis L. (2017). Hearing aid fine-tuning based on Dutch descriptions. International Journal of Audiology, 56(7), 507–515. 10.1080/14992027.2017.1288302
    1. Valentine S., Dundas J. A., Fitz K. (2011). Evidence for the use of a new patient-centered fitting tool. Hearing Review, 18(4), 28–34,
    1. van Esch T. E. M., Kollmeier B., Vormann M., Lyzenga J., Houtgast T., Hällgren M., Larsby B., Athalye S. P., Lutman M. E., Dreschler W. A. (2013). Evaluation of the preliminary auditory profile test battery in an international multi-centre study. International Journal of Audiology, 52(5), 305–321. 10.3109/14992027.2012.759665
    1. Walden B. E., Surr R. K., Grant K. W., Van Summers W., Cord M. T., Dyrlund O. (2005). Effect of signal-to-noise ratio on directional microphone benefit and preference. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 16(9), 662–676. 10.3766/jaaa.16.9.4
    1. Walravens E., Keidser G., Hickson L. (2016). Provision, perception and use of trainable hearing aids in Australia: A survey of clinicians and hearing impaired adults. International Journal of Audiology, 55(12), 787–795. 10.1080/14992027.2016.1219776
    1. Wu Y.-H. (2010). Effect of age on directional microphone hearing aid benefit and preference. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 21(2), 78–89. 10.3766/jaaa.21.2.3
    1. Zimmermann P., Fimm B. (2014). Test of Attentional Performance (Mobility version) Version 1.3. Herzogenrath: Psytest.

Source: PubMed

3
Abonneren